Skip to main content
Advertisement

Main menu

  • Home
  • Content
    • Current Issue
    • Publication Preview--Ahead of Print
    • Past Issue Archive
    • Case of the Week Archive
    • Classic Case Archive
    • Case of the Month Archive
    • COVID-19 Content and Resources
  • For Authors
  • About Us
    • About AJNR
    • Editors
    • American Society of Neuroradiology
  • Submit a Manuscript
  • Podcasts
    • Subscribe on iTunes
    • Subscribe on Stitcher
  • More
    • Subscribers
    • Permissions
    • Advertisers
    • Alerts
    • Feedback
  • Other Publications
    • ajnr

User menu

  • Subscribe
  • Alerts
  • Log in

Search

  • Advanced search
American Journal of Neuroradiology
American Journal of Neuroradiology

American Journal of Neuroradiology

  • Subscribe
  • Alerts
  • Log in

Advanced Search

  • Home
  • Content
    • Current Issue
    • Publication Preview--Ahead of Print
    • Past Issue Archive
    • Case of the Week Archive
    • Classic Case Archive
    • Case of the Month Archive
    • COVID-19 Content and Resources
  • For Authors
  • About Us
    • About AJNR
    • Editors
    • American Society of Neuroradiology
  • Submit a Manuscript
  • Podcasts
    • Subscribe on iTunes
    • Subscribe on Stitcher
  • More
    • Subscribers
    • Permissions
    • Advertisers
    • Alerts
    • Feedback
  • Follow AJNR on Twitter
  • Visit AJNR on Facebook
  • Follow AJNR on Instagram
  • Join AJNR on LinkedIn
  • RSS Feeds
Research ArticleFunctional
Open Access

Association between Resting-State Coactivation in the Parieto-Frontal Network and Intelligence during Late Childhood and Adolescence

C. Li and L. Tian
American Journal of Neuroradiology June 2014, 35 (6) 1150-1156; DOI: https://doi.org/10.3174/ajnr.A3850
C. Li
aFrom the Department of Biomedical Engineering, School of Computer and Information Technology, Beijing Jiaotong University, Beijing, China.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
L. Tian
aFrom the Department of Biomedical Engineering, School of Computer and Information Technology, Beijing Jiaotong University, Beijing, China.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Supplemental
  • Info & Metrics
  • References
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

BACKGROUND AND PURPOSE: A number of studies have associated the adult intelligence quotient with the structure and function of the bilateral parieto-frontal networks, whereas the relationship between intelligence quotient and parieto-frontal network function has been found to be relatively weak in early childhood. Because both human intelligence and brain function undergo protracted development into adulthood, the purpose of the present study was to provide a better understanding of the development of the parieto-frontal network–intelligence quotient relationship.

MATERIALS AND METHODS: We performed independent component analysis of resting-state fMRI data of 84 children and 50 adolescents separately and then correlated full-scale intelligence quotient with the spatial maps of the bilateral parieto-frontal networks of each group.

RESULTS: In children, significant positive spatial-map versus intelligence quotient correlations were detected in the right angular gyrus and inferior frontal gyrus in the right parieto-frontal network, and no significant correlation was observed in the left parieto-frontal network. In adolescents, significant positive correlation was detected in the left inferior frontal gyrus in the left parieto-frontal network, and the correlations in the frontal pole in the 2 parieto-frontal networks were only marginally significant.

CONCLUSIONS: The present findings not only support the critical role of the parieto-frontal networks for intelligence but indicate that the relationship between intelligence quotient and the parieto-frontal network in the right hemisphere has been well established in late childhood, and that the relationship in the left hemisphere was also established in adolescence.

ABBREVIATIONS:

IQ
intelligence quotient
FSIQ
full-scale intelligence quotient
PFN
parieto-frontal network
RS-fMRI
resting-state fMRI
TC-GICA
temporal-concatenation group independent component analysis
R.PFN
right PFN
L.PFN
left PFN
IFG
inferior frontal gyrus

The brain basis of intelligence has been widely investigated for many years.1 A range of neuroimaging paradigms, including MR imaging, EEG, DTI, PET, and task-based fMRI, have been used to investigate the neural basis of intelligence quotient (IQ).2 A large percentage of these studies reported that individual differences in IQ are related to the structure or function of the parieto-frontal regions in adults.3⇓⇓–6 For instance, Haier et al3 reported that more gray matter in the parieto-frontal regions was associated with higher IQ on the basis of the MR imaging scans of adults. On the basis of MR imaging of young adults, Colom et al5,6 reported that the structural features of gray matter in the frontal lobes supported fluid, crystallized, and spatial intelligence. Jung and Haier2 reviewed 37 modern neuroimaging studies associated with intelligence and advanced the parieto-frontal integration theory, which indicated the importance of the parieto-frontal regions, along with some temporal and occipital regions, for intelligence.

Resting-state fMRI (RS-fMRI) is rapidly emerging as a powerful tool for in vivo mapping of neural circuitry in the human brain. Several recent studies investigated the neural basis of IQ in adults by use of RS-fMRI.7⇓⇓–10 Song et al7 analyzed the functional connectivities (FCs) associated with the bilateral dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex and reported that the FCs between parieto-frontal regions were particularly associated with intelligence. Van den Heuvel et al8 examined the RS-fMRI data from a complex network perspective and reported that the characteristic path length in parieto-frontal regions was longer in subjects with lower IQ. Wang et al9 reported the association between IQ and regional homogeneity of RS-fMRI signals in the parieto-frontal and other regions in adults. Cole et al10 reported a highly selective relationship between the global brain connectivity of left lateral prefrontal cortex and individual differences in fluid intelligence.

It should be noted that all these brain-intelligence relationship analyses were based on adults. Both mental abilities and brain function have been reported to undergo protracted development from early childhood into adulthood,11 and the higher-order association areas, including the parieto-frontal regions, have been reported to mature last.12,13 This indicates that the association between intelligence and function of the parieto-frontal regions might not have been established in children. Langeslag et al14 noticed this and analyzed the correlation between IQ and the FCs within the parieto-frontal networks (PFNs) across children 6–8 years old. On the basis of RS-fMRI, a significant functional connectivity-versus-IQ correlation was observed only in the right parieto-frontal network (R.PFN) at a relatively loose threshold (P < .05, false discovery rate–corrected for 10 comparisons), and this correlation in the left parieto-frontal network (L.PFN) was not significant (P < .05, false discovery rate–corrected for 3 comparisons) in their study. To date, the developmental route of the PFN-IQ relationship has not been well depicted, with a relatively weak relationship in early childhood,14 well-established relationship in adulthood, and unknown relationship in late childhood and adolescence.

To provide a better understanding of the development of PFN-IQ relationship, we analyzed the association between Wechsler full-scale IQ (FSIQ) and resting-state coactivation in the bilateral PFNs obtained by independent component analysis of RS-fMRI data. Independent component analysis is a powerful data-driven approach for finding independent patterns in multivariate data and one of the strongest statistical computational approaches in the detection of resting state networks.15,16 As compared with the extensively used seed-based functional connectivity analyses, independent component analysis needs no definition of seed regions and thus any possible bias caused by seed-selection in seed-based functional connectivity analyses could be avoided by use of independent component analysis. We performed the study as follows: 1) on the basis of publicly released RS-fMRI data, temporal-concatenation group independent component analysis (TC-GICA) was performed to acquire the group independent component maps for the child group (84 subjects) and adolescent group (50 subjects) separately. Dual regression was then performed to find individual subject-level versions of the network spatial maps; 2) for each group, the components corresponding to the R.PFN and the L.PFN were identified by means of visual inspection, and voxelwise correlations with FSIQ were performed on the spatial maps of both the R.PFN and the L.PFN across subjects.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Subjects

Data were selected from a large sample RS-fMRI dataset publicly released as “The ADHD-200 Sample” in the “1000 Functional Connectomes Project.”17 Subjects who satisfied the following 4 criteria were included in the present study: 1) typically developing subjects: the subjects had no lifetime history of head trauma with loss of consciousness and no history of neurologic illness or serious physical disease; 2) subjects enrolled at and data collected by Beijing Normal University. As compared with those from other centers, the Beijing dataset included the largest sample size of typically developing subjects. We did not use data from multiple centers in consideration of avoiding any possible center-related variations,18 and the present sample size was relatively large (84 children and 50 adolescents); 3) head motion <2.0 mm displacement in any of the x, y, or z directions and 2.0° of any angular motion throughout the resting-state scan; 4) subjects were right-handed, which was evaluated by the 10-item handedness inventory designed by The National Cooperative Research Team for Handedness of China. According to these 4 criteria, 134 subjects were included in this study, and were divided into 1 child group (84 subjects, 44 girls, 8.00–11.99 years old, with a mean age of 10.22 ± 1.11 years) and 1 adolescent group (50 subjects, 14 girls, 12.00–15.99 years old, with a mean age of 13.47 ± 0.81 years).

Dataset

MR imaging data were obtained by use of a Trio 3T scanner (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany). Functional images were collected axially by use of an echo-planar imaging sequence sensitive to blood oxygen level–dependent contrast with the following parameters: 33 sections, 2000/30 ms (TR/TE), 90° (flip angle), 220 × 220 mm (FOV), 64 × 64 (resolution), 3.5/0.7 mm (thickness/gap). The resulting nominal voxel size was 3.44 × 3.44 × 4.50 mm. The functional imaging lasted for 480 seconds. Whole-brain 3D T1-weighted images were then obtained sagittally with the following parameters: 128 sections, 2530/3.39 ms (TR/TE), 1.33/0 mm (thickness/gap), 256 × 256 (resolution), 240 × 240 mm (FOV), 7° (flip angle). The resulting nominal voxel size was 1.07 × 1.07 × 1.33 mm.

Subject FSIQ scores were assessed by use of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Chinese Children-Revised. Because the raw test scores were tailored to exact age, the FSIQ scores used in this study were independent of age. The mean FSIQ score of the 84 children was 119.4 (standard deviation [SD] = 13.4) and the range was 84–153. The mean FSIQ score of the 50 adolescents was 115.2 (SD = 12.7) and the range was 81–135.

RS-fMRI Data Preprocessing

RS-fMRI data preprocessing was performed by use of FSL (http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl). The following processing steps were applied to the RS-fMRI data of each subject: 1) removing the first 5 volumes; 2) correcting for head motion by use of the MCFLIRT tool in FSL; 3) removing the nonbrain tissues with the Brain Extraction Tool; 4) spatial smoothing by use of a Gaussian kernel of full width at half maximum 5 mm; 5) high-pass temporal filtering to remove slow drift (cutoff frequency = 0.01 Hz); 6) registering the subject's RS-fMRI data to his or her high-resolution structural image and then to Montreal Neurological Institute 152 standard space by use of the FLIRT and FNIRT tools in FSL and resampling the subject's registered RS-fMRI data to 2 × 2 × 2-mm resolution. Because the effects associated with head motion and other noise could be ameliorated through the use of independent component analysis and dual regression, no denoising was performed.

TC-GICA and Dual Regression

TC-GICA was carried out on the datasets of the child group and adolescent group separately by use of MELODIC in FSL. Given computational resource limitations, we performed a 2-stage TC-GICA, as was used in the study by Biswal et al,18 as follows: 1) TC-GICA was performed 10 times, each with 20 randomly selected subjects, and the number of components was fixed at 20.16,18 2) A second-stage independent component analysis was then carried out across the 10 runs to extract the 20 spatial maps consistently identified across the 10 runs. Dual regression was then applied to each subject's preprocessed RS-fMRI data to build individual-subject-level spatial maps.19⇓–21 Specifically, for each subject (separately): 1) the formerly obtained 20 group-independent component analysis spatial maps (based on his or her own group dataset) were used as spatial regressors against the subject's 4D RS-fMRI data to estimate the network time-series of each component. Before being fed into the model, each component map was demeaned and normalized. 2) The resulting 20 time-series were then used as temporal regressors against each subject's preprocessed 4D RS-fMRI data, resulting in 1 individual-subject-level spatial map for each component, reflecting how closely the functional data temporally resembled the component time-series at each voxel. Again, each time-series was demeaned and normalized before entering into the regression model.

PFN-IQ Relationship Analyses

PFN-IQ relationship analyses were performed on the spatial maps of the bilateral PFNs of the child group and adolescent group separately. To do this, we first identified the R.PFN and the L.PFN on the basis of visual inspection of the spatial maps generated by TC-GICA: the obviously right-lateralized network covering mainly the right parieto-frontal regions was identified as the R.PFN, and the network covering mainly the parieto-frontal regions in the left hemisphere was identified as the L.PFN. The spatial maps corresponding to the R.PFN/L.PFN were then collected across subjects into a 4D file (the fourth dimension was subject number). Five thousand permutations were finally performed within the corresponding PFN-mask obtained by thresholding the spatial map of the network on the basis of TC-GICA at 5, to test for significant spatial map–versus–FSIQ correlations by use of the randomize command in FSL. Age, sex, and gray matter attenuation (acquired by voxel-based morphometry analysis of the 3D T1-weighted data, as a voxelwise covariate) were taken as covariates and removed. The results were thresholded at P < .05 (threshold-free cluster enhanced in FSL, family-wise error–corrected).22 To provide a comprehensive perspective about the PFN-IQ relationship at the 2 developmental stages, marginally significant correlations that survived a looser threshold of P < .005 (uncorrected) and cluster size >200 mm3 (25 resampled voxels) were also reported.

To test whether the spatial map–versus–FSIQ correlations were specific to the bilateral PFNs, the medial visual network, the auditory-motor network, and the default mode network were selected as control RSNs, and 5000 permutations were also performed on the 3 control networks within the corresponding mask.

RESULTS

The R.PFN of the child group was similar to that of the adolescent group, and the L.PFNs of the 2 groups were also similar (Fig 1). The R.PFN and the L.PFN of each group appeared to be symmetrical. In each PFN, in addition to the ipsilateral parieto-frontal regions, small clusters were also observed in the presupplementary motor area and posterior cingulate cortex, the ipsilateral fusiform, as well as the contralateral parieto-frontal regions and cerebellum. Spatial maps of all 20 components of children and adolescents are shown in On-line Figs 1 and 2, respectively.

Fig 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig 1.

Spatial maps representing the parieto-frontal networks on the basis of temporal-concatenation group independent component analyses.

In the R.PFN of children, significant positive spatial map–versus–FSIQ correlations were found in the right angular gyrus and inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) (P < .05, threshold-free cluster enhanced, family-wise error–corrected), and positive correlations in left cerebellum and another right angular gyrus region were marginally significant (uncorrected P < .005 and cluster size >200 mm3) (Fig 2a–d and Table). No marginally significant negative correlation was found in the R.PFN of children. In the L.PFN of children, no marginally significant correlation (either positive or negative) was found.

Fig 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig 2.

Positive correlations with full-scale intelligence quotient (FSIQ) across subjects. In the right parieto-frontal network (R.PFN) of children, significant correlations were observed in the right angular gyrus (AG) (a) and inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) (b), and marginally significant correlations were observed in the left cerebellum (c) and another right AG region (d). In adolescents, significant correlations were observed in the left IFG in the left PFN (L.PFN) (e), and marginally significant correlations were observed in the left frontal pole (FP) in the L.PFN (f) and right FP in the R.PFN (g). Green color indicates significant correlations and red-yellow color indicates marginally significant correlations. In the scatterplots, the y-coordinate indicates the mean coactivation of voxels exhibiting significant spatial map–versus–FSIQ correlations.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup

Positive correlations with FSIQ across subjects

In adolescents, significant positive correlations with the spatial map of the L.PFN were found in left IFG (P < .05, threshold-free cluster enhanced, family-wise error–corrected), and the positive correlation in the left frontal pole was marginally significant (uncorrected P < .005 and cluster size >200 mm3) (Fig 2e,-f and Table). Marginally significant positive correlation on the basis of the R.PFN was detected in the right frontal pole (Fig 2g and Table). No marginally significant negative correlation was found in the 2 PFNs of adolescents.

No marginally significant spatial map–versus–FSIQ correlation (uncorrected P < .005 and cluster size >200 mm3), either positive or negative, was detected in any of the 3 control RSNs of any group (children and adolescents). This indicates that the spatial map–versus–FSIQ correlations were specific to the PFNs.

DISCUSSION

In this study, we obtained spatial maps by use of independent component analysis decomposition of RS-fMRI data of 84 older children and 50 adolescents and examined whether the PFN-IQ relationship had been established in each group. Only positive spatial map–versus–FSIQ correlations were detected in this study, and the correlations were specific to the PFNs. In children, significant/marginally significant positive correlations were found in the right angular gyrus and IFG and left cerebellum in the R.PFN (Fig 2a–d and Table). In adolescents, significant positive correlations were found in the left IFG in the L.PFN, and the positive correlations in the frontal pole in the 2 PFNs were marginally significant (Fig 2e–g and Table).

The importance of the PFN for IQ has been reported in many pioneering studies by use of diverse neuroimaging techniques.1⇓⇓–4,7,8,23 Specifically, more gray matter in the bilateral parieto-frontal regions has been reported to be associated with higher IQ.3,24 The fractional anisotropy within the bilateral frontal and occipito-parietal regions has been reported to be positively correlated with IQ.25 With task-based fMRI/PET, the parieto-frontal regions have been reported to be active during working memory26 and a variety of reasoning tasks,23 both of which are important for intelligence. On the basis of graph-theoretical network analyses of EEG data, the degree centralities of parieto-frontal regions have been reported to be significantly correlated with intelligence.27 On the basis of RS-fMRI, IQ has been reported to be associated with functional connectivity between parieto-frontal regions,7 regional homogeneity of RS-fMRI signals in parieto-frontal regions,9 global brain connectivity of the left lateral prefrontal cortex,10 and the normalized characteristic path length of parieto-frontal regions.8 In this study, significant/marginally significant spatial map–versus–FSIQ correlations were found in the right angular gyrus and IFG in the R.PFN of children (Fig 2a, b, and d and Table), left IFG and frontal pole in the L.PFN, and right frontal pole in the R.PFN of adolescents (Fig 2e–g and Table). The right IFG has been reported to play important roles in environment monitoring, action planning, numerical ordering, and symbolic arithmetic.28,29 The left IFG has been repeatedly reported to be involved in phonological processing.30,31 Angular gyrus activations have been reported in such situations as spatial attention and orienting, language and semantic processing, and mathematical cognition.32,33 On the basis of meta-analysis, Bludau et al34 reported the involvement of the frontal pole in action selection, working memory, and perception. Each of the aforementioned functions of the 4 regions is critical for intelligence. Taken together, the present findings of significant/marginally significant spatial map–versus–FSIQ correlations in the PFNs (Fig 2 and Table) support the critical role of the PFNs for intelligence.

In this study, marginally significant spatial map–versus–IQ correlation was found in left cerebellum in the R.PFN of children (Fig 2c and Table). In addition to its critical role in motor control, the cerebellum has been repeatedly reported to be involved in some cognitive functions. As for IQ, Frangou et al24 reported significant correlation between IQ and gray matter attenuation in cerebellum. Taki et al35 reported that the regional gray matter volume of cerebellum, together with that in bilateral prefrontal and temporo-parietal regions, was significantly positively correlated with FSIQ. The present finding of marginally significant correlation in the cerebellums of children is consistent with the 2 reports.

Only positive (and no negative) spatial map–versus–FSIQ correlations were found in this study. Formerly, decreased coactivation in certain resting-state networks has been linked to Alzheimer disease,36 obsessive-compulsive disorder,37 and cocaine addiction,38 and increased coactivation in certain resting-state networks has been linked to better performances in response inhibition21 and motor skill learning.39 In these studies, the extent of coactivation was deemed to represent the strength of functional interactions between the regions within the networks. If this were the case, the significant positive spatial map–versus–FSIQ correlations observed in this study may indicate that stronger functional interactions in the PFNs are advantageous for intelligence. According to the influential neural efficiency hypothesis of intelligence,40 brighter individuals would display lower brain activation during cognitive task performance. Limited by datasets (no task data were available), the present results answered only the question about whether the PFN-IQ relationship exists at the 2 developmental stages. Further studies including both task and resting data may make it clear about how the PFN regions are involved in cognition.

The correlation in the R.PFN of children survived a family-wise error–corrected threshold of P < .05, whereas the correlation in the R.PFN of adolescents survived only a looser threshold of P < .005 (uncorrected) and cluster size >200 mm3. This does not mean that the R.PFN-IQ relationship become weaker with development. It should be noted that the sample size of children was 84, and that of adolescents was 50 in this study. Because statistical power decreases with the decrease of sample size, the present finding of marginally significant correlations in adolescents may be interpreted, at least in part, by the smaller sample size of the group. Indeed, in this study, a correlation coefficient of 0.41 (right IFG in the R.PFN) in children corresponded to significant correlation, whereas a correlation of 0.43 (right frontal pole in the R.PFN) in adolescents only corresponded to marginally significant correlation (Table).

As has been mentioned, only a few studies have been performed specially for investigating the functional basis of IQ in children, and, to our knowledge, no study has been performed on the functional basis of IQ in adolescents, though numerous studies have been performed on that in adults. Langeslag et al14 noticed that both the brain and IQ underwent significant development before adulthood and proposed the importance of analyzing whether the PFN-IQ relationship had been established in children. They analyzed the correlation between IQ and the parieto-frontal functional connectivity across children of 6–8 years old, on the basis of RS-fMRI. In their study, significant functional connectivity-versus-IQ correlation was observed only in the R.PFN at a relatively loose threshold (P < .05, false discovery rate–corrected for only 10 comparisons), and the correlation in the L.PFN was not significant (P < .05, false discovery rate–corrected for 3 comparisons). In the present study, the spatial map–versus–FSIQ correlations in the R.PFN of children (Fig 2a,-b and Table) and L.PFN of adolescents (Fig 2e and Table) were strong enough to survive an family-wise error–corrected threshold of P < .05 (threshold-free cluster enhanced, family-wise error–corrected for the number of voxels within the corresponding PFN mask). The present results bridge the gap between the findings that are based on adults and the results by Langeslag et al14 by indicating that during late childhood, the PFN-IQ relationship in the right hemisphere has been well established, and that relationship in the left hemisphere was also established during adolescence.

Both the study by Langeslag et al14 and the present study implicated a right-lateralized advantage in the development of PFN-IQ relationship. One speculative explanation for the findings is that the right hemisphere of a child's brain may develop earlier than its contralateral counterpart, and the left hemisphere may develop later to build a PFN-IQ relationship similar to that in adults. This speculation is consistent with former findings of rightward asymmetries of brain maturation in early developmental stages. For instance, the volume of the right hemisphere has been reported to be significantly larger than that of its left counterpart in children between 4–18 years old.41 Zhu et al42 reported that the FCs in the right hemisphere were stronger than those in the left hemisphere in children of 0–10 years old. Zhou et al43 reported right-lateralized cortical thickness asymmetries in vision- and language-relevant areas in subjects of 5–14 years of age. Further studies are needed to provide a longitudinal view about the development of hemisphere-IQ relationship.

It should be noted that we investigated the PFN-IQ relationship based only on children of 8.00–11.99 years old and adolescents of 12.00–15.99 years old. These age ranges can provide only information about the extent of influences of resting-state coactivation in the PFN on IQ at the 2 developmental stages. To provide an overall view of the development of the PFN-IQ relationship, a study including samples covering a larger age range (eg, from 5 years old to adulthood) is needed. In addition, a larger sample size may enable a finer division of age ranges (eg, every 2 years rather than 4 years, as was used in the current study) and thus facilitate the acquisition of more detailed and precise understanding of the development of the PFN-IQ relationship. Furthermore, comparable sample size at each age range may provide an intuitive idea about the relative strength of the PFN-IQ relationship at different developmental stages.

CONCLUSIONS

We correlated FSIQ with the spatial maps of resting-state PFNs across 84 children and 50 adolescents to provide a better understanding of the development of the PFN-IQ relationship. The spatial map–versus–FSIQ correlations in the R.PFN of children and L.PFN of adolescents were strong enough to survive an family-wise error–corrected threshold of P < .05. The present findings not only supported the critical role of the PFNs for intelligence but bridged the gap between the results that were based on adults and those based on younger children (6–8 years old) by indicating that during late childhood, the PFN-IQ relationship in the right hemisphere has been well established, and that relationship in the left hemisphere was also established during adolescence. Further studies including a larger sample size and covering a larger age range are needed to better depict the developmental trajectory of the PFN-IQ relationship.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors are grateful to the anonymous referees for their significant and constructive comments and suggestions, which greatly improved the study. The authors acknowledge the “1000 Functional Connectomes Project” for providing the dataset.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Grant Nos. 30800249, 61272356) and the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities (Grant Nos. 2011JBM021, 2013JBZ003).

Indicates open access to non-subscribers at www.ajnr.org

References

  1. 1.↵
    1. Colom R,
    2. Jung RE,
    3. Haier RJ
    . Distributed brain sites for the G-factor of intelligence. NeuroImage 2006;31:1359–65
    CrossRefPubMed
  2. 2.↵
    1. Jung RE,
    2. Haier RJ
    . The Parieto-Frontal Integration Theory (P-FIT) of intelligence: converging neuroimaging evidence. Behav Brain Sci 2007;30:135–54
    CrossRefPubMed
  3. 3.↵
    1. Haier RJ,
    2. Jung RE,
    3. Yeo RA,
    4. et al
    . Structural brain variation and general intelligence. NeuroImage 2004;23:425–33
    CrossRefPubMed
  4. 4.↵
    1. Jung RE,
    2. Haier RJ,
    3. Yeo RA,
    4. et al
    . Sex differences in N-acetylaspartate correlates of general intelligence: a 1H-MRS study of normal human brain. NeuroImage 2005;26:965–72
    CrossRefPubMed
  5. 5.↵
    1. Colom R,
    2. Haier RJ,
    3. Head K,
    4. et al
    . Gray matter correlates of fluid, crystallized, and spatial intelligence: testing the P-Fit model. Intelligence 2009;37:124–35
    CrossRef
  6. 6.↵
    1. Colom R,
    2. Burgaleta M,
    3. Roman FJ,
    4. et al
    . Neuroanatomic overlap between intelligence and cognitive factors: morphometry methods provide support for the key role of the frontal lobes. NeuroImage 2013;72:143–52
    CrossRefPubMed
  7. 7.↵
    1. Song M,
    2. Zhou Y,
    3. Li J,
    4. et al
    . Brain spontaneous functional connectivity and intelligence. NeuroImage 2008;41:1168–76
    CrossRefPubMed
  8. 8.↵
    1. van den Heuvel MP,
    2. Stam CJ,
    3. Kahn RS,
    4. et al
    . Efficiency of functional brain networks and intellectual performance. J Neurosci 2009;29:7619–24
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  9. 9.↵
    1. Wang L,
    2. Song M,
    3. Jiang T,
    4. et al
    . Regional homogeneity of the resting-state brain activity correlates with individual intelligence. Neurosci Lett 2011;488:275–78
    CrossRefPubMed
  10. 10.↵
    1. Cole MW,
    2. Yarkoni T,
    3. Repovs G,
    4. et al
    . Global connectivity of prefrontal cortex predicts cognitive control and intelligence. J Neurosci 2012;32:8988–99
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  11. 11.↵
    1. Casey BJ,
    2. Tottenham N,
    3. Liston C,
    4. et al
    . Imaging the developing brain: what have we learned about cognitive development? Trends Cogn Sci 2005;9:104–10
    CrossRefPubMed
  12. 12.↵
    1. Gogtay N,
    2. Giedd JN,
    3. Lusk L,
    4. et al
    . Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2004;101:8174–79
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  13. 13.↵
    1. Sowell ER,
    2. Thompson PM,
    3. Leonard CM,
    4. et al
    . Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and brain growth in normal children. J Neurosci 2004;24:8223–31
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  14. 14.↵
    1. Langeslag SJ,
    2. Schmidt M,
    3. Ghassabian A,
    4. et al
    . Functional connectivity between parietal and frontal brain regions and intelligence in young children: the generation R study. Hum Brain Mapp 2013;34:3299–307
    CrossRefPubMed
  15. 15.↵
    1. Beckmann CF,
    2. DeLuca M,
    3. Devlin JT,
    4. et al
    . Investigations into resting-state connectivity using independent component analysis. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2005;360:1001–13
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  16. 16.↵
    1. Smith SM,
    2. Fox PT,
    3. Miller KL,
    4. et al
    . Correspondence of the brain's functional architecture during activation and rest. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2009;106:13040–45
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  17. 17.↵
    ADHD-200 Sample. http://fcon_1000.projects.nitrc.org/indi/adhd200/. Accessed Jan 26, 2013
  18. 18.↵
    1. Biswal BB,
    2. Mennes M,
    3. Zuo XN,
    4. et al
    . Toward discovery science of human brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2010;107:4734–39
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  19. 19.↵
    1. Filippini N,
    2. MacIntosh BJ,
    3. Hough MG,
    4. et al
    . Distinct patterns of brain activity in young carriers of the apoE-epsilon4 allele. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2009;106:7209–14
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  20. 20.↵
    1. Beckmann C,
    2. Mackay C,
    3. Filippini N,
    4. et al
    . Group comparison of resting-state fMRI data using multi-subject ICA and dual regression. In: 15th Annual Meeting of Organization for Hum Brain Mapp, poster 441 SU-AM. 2009
  21. 21.↵
    1. Tian L,
    2. Kong Y,
    3. Ren J,
    4. et al
    . Spatial vs temporal features in ICA of resting-state fMRI: a quantitative and qualitative investigation in the context of response inhibition. PLoS One 2013;8:e66572
    CrossRefPubMed
  22. 22.↵
    1. Smith SM,
    2. Nichols TE
    . Threshold-free cluster enhancement: addressing problems of smoothing, threshold dependence and localisation in cluster inference. NeuroImage 2009;44:83–98
    CrossRefPubMed
  23. 23.↵
    1. Fangmeier T,
    2. Knauff M,
    3. Ruff CC,
    4. et al
    . FMRI evidence for a three-stage model of deductive reasoning. J Cogn Neurosci 2006;18:320–34
    CrossRefPubMed
  24. 24.↵
    1. Frangou S,
    2. Chitins X,
    3. Williams SC
    . Mapping IQ and gray matter density in healthy young people. NeuroImage 2004;23:800–05
    CrossRefPubMed
  25. 25.↵
    1. Schmithorst VJ,
    2. Wilke M,
    3. Dardzinski BJ,
    4. et al
    . Cognitive functions correlate with white matter architecture in a normal pediatric population: a diffusion tensor MR imaging study. Hum Brain Mapp 2005;26:139–47
    CrossRefPubMed
  26. 26.↵
    1. Gray WD,
    2. Schoelles MJ,
    3. Bringsjord SA,
    4. et al
    . SAGE: Five powerful ideas for studying and transforming the intelligence analyst's task environment. In: 47th Annual Conference of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society 2003:1019–23
  27. 27.↵
    1. Langer N,
    2. Pedroni A,
    3. Gianotti LR,
    4. et al
    . Functional brain network efficiency predicts intelligence. Hum Brain Mapp 2012;33:1393–406
    CrossRefPubMed
  28. 28.↵
    1. Knops A,
    2. Willmes K
    . Numerical ordering and symbolic arithmetic share frontal and parietal circuits in the right hemisphere. NeuroImage 2013;84C:786–95
    PubMed
  29. 29.↵
    1. Verbruggen F,
    2. Aron AR,
    3. Stevens MA,
    4. et al
    . Theta burst stimulation dissociates attention and action updating in human inferior frontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2010;107:13966–71
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  30. 30.↵
    1. Bitan T,
    2. Cheon J,
    3. Lu D,
    4. et al
    . Developmental changes in activation and effective connectivity in phonological processing. NeuroImage 2007;38:564–75
    CrossRefPubMed
  31. 31.↵
    1. MacSweeney M,
    2. Brammer MJ,
    3. Waters D,
    4. et al
    . Enhanced activation of the left inferior frontal gyrus in deaf and dyslexic adults during rhyming. Brain 2009;132:1928–40
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  32. 32.↵
    1. Liang P,
    2. Wang Z,
    3. Yang Y,
    4. et al
    . Three subsystems of the inferior parietal cortex are differently affected in mild cognitive impairment. J Alzheimers Dis 2012;30:475–87
    PubMed
  33. 33.↵
    1. Esposito R,
    2. Mosca A,
    3. Pieramico V,
    4. et al
    . Characterization of resting state activity in MCI individuals. Peer J 2013;1:e135
    CrossRef
  34. 34.↵
    1. Bludau S,
    2. Eickhoff SB,
    3. Mohlberg H,
    4. et al
    . Cytoarchitecture, probability maps and functions of the human frontal pole. NeuroImage 2014;93(Pt 2):260–75
    CrossRefPubMed
  35. 35.↵
    1. Taki Y,
    2. Hashizume H,
    3. Sassa Y,
    4. et al
    . Correlation among body height, intelligence, and brain gray matter volume in healthy children. NeuroImage 2012;59:1023–27
    CrossRefPubMed
  36. 36.↵
    1. Greicius MD,
    2. Srivastava G,
    3. Reiss AL,
    4. et al
    . Default-mode network activity distinguishes Alzheimer's disease from healthy aging: evidence from functional MRI. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2004;101:4637–42
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  37. 37.↵
    1. Cheng Y,
    2. Xu J,
    3. Nie B,
    4. et al
    . Abnormal resting-state activities and functional connectivities of the anterior and the posterior cortexes in medication-naive patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. PLoS One 2013;8:e67478
    CrossRefPubMed
  38. 38.↵
    1. Cisler JM,
    2. Elton A,
    3. Kennedy AP,
    4. et al
    . Altered functional connectivity of the insular cortex across prefrontal networks in cocaine addiction. Psychiatry Res 2013;213:39–46
    CrossRefPubMed
  39. 39.↵
    1. Ma L,
    2. Narayana S,
    3. Robin DA,
    4. et al
    . Changes occur in resting state network of motor system during 4 weeks of motor skill learning. NeuroImage 2011;58:226–33
    CrossRefPubMed
  40. 40.↵
    1. Neubauer AC,
    2. Fink A
    . Intelligence and neural efficiency. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2009;33:1004–23
    CrossRefPubMed
  41. 41.↵
    1. Giedd JN,
    2. Snell JW,
    3. Lange N,
    4. et al
    . Quantitative magnetic resonance imaging of human brain development: ages 4–18. Cereb Cortex 1996;6:551–60
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  42. 42.↵
    1. Zhu C,
    2. Guo X,
    3. Jin Z,
    4. et al
    . Influences of brain development and ageing on cortical interactive networks. Clin Neurophysiol 2011;122:278–83
    CrossRefPubMed
  43. 43.↵
    1. Zhou D,
    2. Lebel C,
    3. Evans A,
    4. et al
    . Cortical thickness asymmetry from childhood to older adulthood. NeuroImage 2013;83:66–74
    CrossRefPubMed
  • Received September 3, 2013.
  • Accepted after revision November 11, 2013.
  • © 2014 by American Journal of Neuroradiology
View Abstract
PreviousNext
Back to top

In this issue

American Journal of Neuroradiology: 35 (6)
American Journal of Neuroradiology
Vol. 35, Issue 6
1 Jun 2014
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
  • Complete Issue (PDF)
Advertisement
Print
Download PDF
Email Article

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on American Journal of Neuroradiology.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Association between Resting-State Coactivation in the Parieto-Frontal Network and Intelligence during Late Childhood and Adolescence
(Your Name) has sent you a message from American Journal of Neuroradiology
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the American Journal of Neuroradiology web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Citation Tools
Association between Resting-State Coactivation in the Parieto-Frontal Network and Intelligence during Late Childhood and Adolescence
C. Li, L. Tian
American Journal of Neuroradiology Jun 2014, 35 (6) 1150-1156; DOI: 10.3174/ajnr.A3850

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Share
Association between Resting-State Coactivation in the Parieto-Frontal Network and Intelligence during Late Childhood and Adolescence
C. Li, L. Tian
American Journal of Neuroradiology Jun 2014, 35 (6) 1150-1156; DOI: 10.3174/ajnr.A3850
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One
Purchase

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • ABBREVIATIONS:
    • MATERIALS AND METHODS
    • RESULTS
    • DISCUSSION
    • CONCLUSIONS
    • ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Supplemental
  • Info & Metrics
  • References
  • PDF

Related Articles

  • PubMed
  • Google Scholar

Cited By...

  • Functional connectivity directionality between large-scale resting-state networks in children and adolescence from the Healthy Brain Network sample
  • What is an adaptive pattern of brain network coupling for a child? It depends on their environment
  • Abnormal coactivation of the hypothalamus and salience network in patients with cluster headache
  • Crossref
  • Google Scholar

This article has not yet been cited by articles in journals that are participating in Crossref Cited-by Linking.

More in this TOC Section

  • Connectomic Basis for Tremor Control in Stereotactic Radiosurgical Thalamotomy
  • Reliability of Functional and Diffusion MR Imaging Near Cerebral Cavernous Malformations
  • Topological Structural Brain Connectivity Alterations in Aspartylglucosaminuria: A Case-Control Study
Show more Functional

Similar Articles

Advertisement

News and Updates

  • Lucien Levy Best Research Article Award
  • Thanks to our 2022 Distinguished Reviewers
  • Press Releases

Resources

  • Evidence-Based Medicine Level Guide
  • How to Participate in a Tweet Chat
  • AJNR Podcast Archive
  • Ideas for Publicizing Your Research
  • Librarian Resources
  • Terms and Conditions

Opportunities

  • Share Your Art in Perspectives
  • Get Peer Review Credit from Publons
  • Moderate a Tweet Chat

American Society of Neuroradiology

  • Neurographics
  • ASNR Annual Meeting
  • Fellowship Portal
  • Position Statements

© 2023 by the American Society of Neuroradiology | Print ISSN: 0195-6108 Online ISSN: 1936-959X

Powered by HighWire