Elsevier

Epilepsy & Behavior

Volume 3, Issue 4, August 2002, Pages 309-315
Epilepsy & Behavior

Review
The frontal lobe role in memory: a review of convergent evidence and implications for the Wada memory test

https://doi.org/10.1016/S1525-5050(02)00039-2Get rights and content

Abstract

Functional imaging studies have implicated the frontal lobe in many of the memory processes often thought to be the domain of medial temporal structures. Results from fMRI studies of normal subjects have suggested that some components of memory formation, including those components tested during the Wada memory test, may involve frontal lobe regions. Specific behavioral disruptions during carotid amytal injections support a model for frontal lobe anesthesia in explaining results of the Wada memory test. Cortical stimulation data suggest that frontal lobe disruption is sufficient to cause memory disturbances. The convergence of evidence suggests frontal lobe memory may limit the predictive value of the Wada memory test in defining the risk of memory loss following temporal lobectomy.

Introduction

Recent advances in functional imaging, especially functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), have refocused attention on the role of the frontal lobes in memory processes [1]. Additional evidence from lesion studies, and from cortical stimulation mapping, supports such roles for the frontal lobe. Results from material-by-hemisphere interactions when different stimuli are presented during the Wada memory test are also reviewed. The collective data and anatomic basis for the Wada test leave open the possibility that the results of the Wada memory test may be driven by impairment of frontal lobe function. Portions of this review have been presented in abstract form [2].

Section snippets

Functional imaging of frontal and medial temporal regions

Attempts to localize anatomical substrates for memory processes have historically focused on the role of the medial temporal lobe (MTL). This broad anatomical term incorporates the hippocampus, parahippocampal gyrus, entorhinal cortex, and, in some uses, the amygdala. The severe amnesia observed following bilateral MTL injury, as in the famous case of H.M., who underwent surgical removal of both MTLs [3], has left no doubt as to the importance of this area in memory. Injury to other cortical

Preoperative prediction of memory loss: the cerebral amytal (Wada) test

Memory loss following neurosurgical procedures involving resection of the medial temporal lobes remains a feared complication. A common cause of medically intractable seizure disorders is mesial temporal sclerosis. Patients who suffer from epilepsy due to mesial temporal sclerosis may be candidates for surgical treatment if seizures can be demonstrated to arise from one temporal lobe. Surgery for intractable seizures requires extensive resection of medial temporal lobe structures, including

The Wada memory test

Two major methods exist for testing memory preoperatively with the Wada test [14]. Both methods involve the presentation of items following intracarotid injection of sodium amytal. After injection, the hemisphere injected becomes anesthetized. In the first method (the “Seattle” test), visually presented, namable objects are presented. Testing begins prior to injection of amytal. Patients are presented visual line drawings of objects and instructed to name them. A written sentence is then

Limitations of the Wada test

In addition to anatomical questions about the validity of the Wada test to assess medial temporal lobe function, the documented predictive value of the Wada test is suboptimal. When postoperative neuropsychological tests are performed, the cerebral amytal test is found to be only partially predictive of postoperative verbal memory deficits [14]. Although severe global amnesia can be avoided if patients pass the cerebral amytal test prior to resection of medial temporal structures, significant

Areas of the brain anesthetized by the Wada test

Not only does the Wada test not accurately predict the effects of MTL removal, additional studies suggest the metabolic changes from amytal injection may not primarily involve MTL structures [15]. Medial temporal lobe structures receive perfusion from both the anterior (carotid) and posterior (vertebrobasilar) circulation [16]. Branches of the middle cerebral and anterior choroidal artery perfuse anterior medial temporal lobe structures, with posterior regions arising from branches of the

The frontal lobe affects memory when injured

Although many regions of cortex, including lateral temporal and parietal cortex, may also contribute to effects of an intracarotid amytal injection, the frontal lobe is, for reasons mentioned, especially of interest in considering effects of memory disruption. Lesion data, although classically focusing on frontal lobe functions such as speech production, response selection, and executive functions [1], also supports a role for frontal lobe in memory. Impairment of source memory [26],

Lateralization of memory functions: the Wada test and the frontal lobes

Thus, if disruption of frontal lobe function is a plausible source for memory changes seen during the Wada test, the lateralization of memory disruption should follow changes seen in frontal lobe activation for different tests. One paradigm used in the neuroimaging literature, encoding of words or faces during fMRI tasks, reveals very strong lateralization of frontal lobe activation in normal subjects [9], with poor lateralization of activation seen for medial temporal regions. If memory

Is disruption of the frontal lobe sufficient?

Although the results of lateralization of memory formation during the Wada test are suggestive of frontal lobe involvement, and lesion literature suggests a role for the frontal lobe and memory, it is not clear that an acute “lesion” of the frontal lobe should cause the disruption in memory seen during the Wada testing. Another model of frontal lobe disruption, which provides anatomic specificity, is cortical stimulation. If focal frontal lobe disruption is sufficient to cause memory

Correlating fMRI and Wada test

In a separate patient, with left frontal seizures by surface recordings, Wada testing lateralized language to the left and verbal memory to the right (based on Montreal testing of verbal encoding). In the setting of this unusual dissociation, a fMRI was obtained (below) during tasks that activate both speech areas (e.g., classic Broca’s area, inferior frontal gyrus) and the more dorsolateral frontal area discussed above as a region involved in memory. In this patient, inferior frontal

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by NIH grant K23 NS41272.

References (34)

  • R.L. Buckner et al.

    Functional anatomical studies of explicit and implicit memory retrieval tasks

    J. Neurosci.

    (1995)
  • K.B. McDermott et al.

    Direct comparison of episodic encoding and retrieval of words: an event-related fMRI study

    Memory

    (1999)
  • J. Brewer et al.

    Making memories: activity that predicts how well visual experience will be remembered

    Science

    (1998)
  • A.D. Wagner et al.

    Material specific lateralization of prefrontal activation during episodic encoding and retrieval

    NeuroReport

    (1998)
  • A.D. Wagner et al.

    Building memories: remembering and forgetting of verbal experiences as predicted by brain activity

    Science

    (1998)
  • C. Branch et al.

    Intracarotid sodium amytal for the lateralization of cerebral speech dominance

    J. Neurosurg.

    (1964)
  • B.G. Kim et al.

    Interpretation of Wada memory test for lateralization of seizure focus by use of 99Tc-HMPAO SPECT

    Epilepsia

    (2000)
  • Cited by (20)

    • Female verbal memory advantage in temporal, but not frontal lobe epilepsy

      2018, Epilepsy Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      The temporal lobe and the hippocampus are crucial for episodic memory (Squire and Zola-Morgan, 1997). In addition, frontal lobe structures may support memory encoding and retrieval through organizational and controlling processes (Fletcher and Henson, 2001; Ojemann and Kelley, 2002). For instance, semantic clustering during word list recall is related to frontal lobe functioning (Long et al., 2010; Manning et al., 2012; Savage et al., 2001).

    • Validity of the WADA test [3]

      2002, Epilepsy and Behavior
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text