Restricted sleep and negative affective states in commercial pilots during short haul operations

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2011.09.031Get rights and content

Abstract

This study aims to investigate (1) the relationship between restricted sleep and Heightened Emotional Activity (HEA) during normal flight operations, and (2) whether sleep patterns influence the strength of the HEA as a response to threats. Accident investigation reports continue to highlight the relationship between restricted sleep and poor safety outcomes. However, to date we have a limited understanding of how sleep and HEA interact. A total of 302 sectors of normal airline flight operations were observed by trained observers, and instances of heightened emotional activity were recorded. During the cruise phase of each of these sectors, crew members were asked to calculate the amount of sleep they had obtained in previous 24 and 48 h. In the 302 sectors of normal flight operations, 535 instances of HEA were observed. Descriptive analyses of instances of HEA and sleep in the prior 24 and 48 h showed a significant relationship between the occurrence of HEA and recent sleep. The relationship between restricted sleep and HEA suggests that there may well be further implications with respect to operational safety.

Highlights

► We conducted a Line Operations Safety Audit with a commercial airline. ► We observed 302 operational sectors and noted emotional activity on the flight deck. ► We interviewed the flight crews about prior sleep and wake hours. ► We established a relationship between restricted sleep and emotional responses.

Introduction

Optimal performance is critical in high-risk environments such as aviation. Factors in the operating environment such as fatigue can degrade performance (Belenky et al., 2003, Thomas and Ferguson, 2010). Matthews and Desmond (2002) define fatigue as a transient state associated with difficulties in maintaining task-directed effort and attention during sustained performance. A high external load, compounded by fatigue, may impact an individual's affective response to operational threats. The fight or flight reactions associated with affective responses have evolved as part of our biologically ‘hardwired’ survival skills (Caruso and Salovey, 2004, Ledoux, 1998, Damasio, 1994). Heightened Emotional Activity (HEA) is defined as the affective responses to perceived environmental threats encountered (Drury et al., 2010). Expressed in the form of auditory communication and body language, HEAs are “biological responses to changes in perception as to the nature or level of threats to the environment” (Drury et al., 2010). Heightened Emotional Activity has implications for operational performance as higher-order cognitive processing is impaired, and physiological activation increases as a reaction to fatigue induced emotional responses (Angus et al., 1985, Pigeau et al., 1995).

Airline flight crews are a group that are exposed to the physiological and cognitive decline associated with extended periods of stress and fatigue. The normal flight deck environment can operate between the calm of cruise flight and the intensity of the descent and approach to land. Short-haul airline operations generally operate multiple take-off and landings as part of a 24/7 operation, sometimes with short layovers (Gander et al., 1998a). Long-haul airline operations are conducted over longer flight sequences, generally with extended layovers in a different time zone disrupting circadian rhythms (Samel et al., 1997, Gander et al., 1998b). Both operational schedules are therefore associated with disrupted sleep patterns and fatigue issues.

Fatigue has been identified as a major contributor to aviation accidents, as documented in aviation accident reports. American International Airways Flight 808 stalled and crashed while attempting to land at the US Naval air station at Guantanamo Bay Cuba (NTSB, 1993). In the analysis of the accident, the National Transportation Safety Board (NTSB) reports that the captain's fatigue and stress degraded his situational awareness to the point where he was unable to register the concerns from the other crew members that “they were not going to make it” while he continued the unstable, and more difficult, approach to runway 10 (Rosekind et al., 1993). The captain had been awake for 23.5 h and had slept for five of the 28.5 h prior to the accident (Rosekind et al., 1993).

Professionals working in high-risk environments are generally quite capable of mitigating emotions, fatigue, and performance issues as they arise. Emotive moments are by nature short-term and can be dealt with as they occur, but the intensity of the initial emotion does not linger. Fatigue issues can be resolved with quality rest and sleep. Performance issues may be short in duration, such as those associated with flying in adverse weather, or corrected simply through more flight time to improve the skill sets necessary to deal with such issues. The pairing of any two of the three processes may not present insurmountable difficulties for individual management. Problems are more likely to arise when emotions, fatigue, and performance challenges occur simultaneously. Using the example described above, the crew of American International Airways Flight 808 exhibited all the signs of fatigue, heightened emotion, and performance degradation during the accident sequence.

The aim of the current study was to examine the relationship between restricted sleep and heightened emotional activity using recent sleep history (i.e. sleep in the prior 24 h and 48 h) and observable markers of emotional activity.

Section snippets

Sample

Trained observers collected data in 2009 during 302 normal flight sectors of commercial airline jet operations. All flight sectors were crewed by a Captain and a First Officer. Average flight time for Captains was 15.8 years of flying (SD = 8.3). First Officers had an average of 8.3 of flying (SD = 6.8). Due to strict anonymity involved with data collection, of this nature, it was not possible to determine if an individual pilot was observed more than once. The sectors were chosen to represent the

Results

Captains sleep in the previous 24 h ranged 3 to 10 h of sleep with an average of 7.04 and a Standard Deviation of 1.18. First Officers ranged from 3 to 13 h of sleep with an average of 7.11 and a Standard Deviation of 1.43. Captains sleep in the previous 48 h ranged from 9 to 18 h of sleep with an average of 14.37 and a Standard Deviation of 1.81. First Officers ranged from 9 to 20 h of sleep with an average of 14.27 and a Standard Deviation of 1.99. Captains awake hours prior to sector ranged from

Discussion

This paper presents an exploratory investigation into the relationship between restricted sleep and Heightened Emotional Activity (HEA) as a response to threats encountered during normal operations. The findings demonstrate that when crew members’ sleep was restricted, there was an increase in HEA. Given the demonstrated relationships between restricted sleep and HEA, the findings have implications for the design and development of fatigue risk management systems in high-risk industries such as

Conclusion

It is operationally significant that this study has shown that crews with restricted sleep display emotional responses. The findings indicate that reduced sleep was associated with increased occurrences of confusion and frustration in response to threats. This has implications for performance on the flight deck suggesting that crews need to be aware of the potential risks associated with emotions and fatigue.

Additional training in how to detect and regulate the emotional response to a threat

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank the airline that participated in this study, and the individual crews who not only volunteered information about their personal histories but also allowed observers to analyse their performance during normal flight operations. We acknowledge their contributions to the science of fatigue, emotions, and performance.

References (35)

  • R. Cowie et al.

    Describing the emotional states that are expressed in speech

    Speech Communications

    (2003)
  • J.J. Radley et al.

    Repeated stress and structural plasticity in the brain

    Ageing Research Reviews

    (2005)
  • A. Samel et al.

    Aircrew fatigue in long-haul operations

    Accident Analysis and Prevention

    (1997)
  • K.R. Scherer

    Vocal communication of emotion: a review of research paradigms

    Speech Communication

    (2003)
  • R.G. Angus et al.

    Effects of prolonged sleep deprivation, with and without chronic physical exercise, on mood and performance

    Psychophysiology

    (1985)
  • G. Belenky et al.

    Pattens of performance degradation and restoration during sleep restriction and subsequent recovery: a sleep dose study

    Journal of Sleep Research

    (2003)
  • A. Ben-Zeev

    The nature of emotions

    Philisophical Studies

    (1987)
  • S.A. Bennett

    Flight crew stress and fatigue in low-cost commercial air operations: an appraisal

    International Journal of Risk Assessment and Management

    (2003)
  • J.A. Caldwell et al.

    The effects of 37 hours of continuous wakefulness on the psychological arousal, cognitive performance, self-reported mood, and simulator flight performance of f-117a pilots

    Military Psychology

    (2004)
  • D.R. Caruso et al.

    The Emotionally Intelligent Manager: How to Develop and Use the Four Key Emotional Skills of Leadership

    (2004)
  • A.R. Damasio

    Descartes’ Error—Emotion, Reason and the Human Brain

    (1994)
  • D.A. Drury et al.

    Effects of restricted sleep on heightened emotional activity

  • P. Ekman

    An argument for basic emotions

    Cognition and Emotion

    (1992)
  • J.P Forgas

    Mood and judgement: the affective infusion model (aim)

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1995)
  • N.H. Frijda

    The Emotions

    (1986)
  • P.H. Gander et al.

    Flight crew fatigue II: short-haul air transport operations

    Aviation, Space and Environmental Medicine

    (1998)
  • P.H. Gander et al.

    Flight crew fatigue V: long-haul air transport operations

    Aviation, Space and Environmental Medicine

    (1998)
  • Cited by (25)

    • Automatic drowsiness detection for safety-critical operations using ensemble models and EEG signals

      2022, Process Safety and Environmental Protection
      Citation Excerpt :

      Hence, human reliability continues to play an important role in critical safety tasks and human factors (e.g. poor rest habits, reactions to medicines, and sleep deprivation) or factors task-related (e.g., shift night, stress, and monotony) are associated, for example, with fatigue, reducing overall employee performance in work environments and leading to drowsiness (Kariuki and Löwe, 2006; B and Chinara, 2021). Indeed, drowsiness, commonly linked to accidents with drivers, is also investigated in emotional activity during flight operations and response to threats (Drury et al., 2012) and in real-time fatigue monitoring of forestry workers (Bowen et al., 2019) as it is presented in the daily lives of professionals. In O&G, Waage et al. (2012) analyzed drowsiness during three different shift schedules in oil rig workers during the few days and swing shifts.

    • Sleep, Health, and Society

      2022, Sleep Medicine Clinics
      Citation Excerpt :

      Airline pilots face similar challenges, in addition to challenges presented by crossing many time zones. To address these concerns, sleep disorders screening in addition to circadian approaches and scheduled napping have shown effectiveness in improving safety.189–197 Among law enforcement and first responders, several studies have shown that sleep disturbances are common among police officers198–203 and firefighters.204,205

    • Sleep, Health, and Society

      2020, Sleep Medicine Clinics
    • Fatigue in aviation: A systematic review of the literature

      2020, International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text