Elsevier

Neuropsychologia

Volume 47, Issue 14, December 2009, Pages 3045-3058
Neuropsychologia

Reviews and perspectives
Stroke and episodic memory disorders

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.08.002Get rights and content

Abstract

Memory impairments are common after stroke, and the anatomical basis for impairments may be quite variable. To determine the range of stroke-related memory impairment, we identified all case reports and group studies through the Medline database and the Science Citation Index. There is no hypothesis about memory that is unique to stroke, but there are several important facets of memory impairment after stroke: (1) Every node of the limbic system implicated in memory may be damaged by stroke but very rarely in isolation and the combination of amnesia with the associated deficits often illuminates additional aspects of memory functions. (2) Stroke produces amnesia by damage to critical convergence white matter connections of the limbic system, and stroke is the only etiology of amnesia that can delineate the entire pathway of memory and critical convergence points. (3) Stroke also impairs memory, without causing classical amnesia, by damaging brain regions responsible for cognitive processes, some modality specific and some more generally strategic, that are essential for normal learning and recall.

Introduction

The fundamental organization of the limbic structures essential for normal learning and recall has been known for decades. Anatomical studies in nonhuman primates and rodents (Aggleton et al., 1987, Aggleton and Mishkin, 1984, De Olmos, 1990, Jolkkonen et al., 2002, Nauta, 1961, Roberts et al., 2007, Rosene and Van Hoesen, 1987, Russchen et al., 1985, Swanson, 1978, Swanson and Cowan, 1975, Swanson and Cowan, 1977, Zola-Morgan et al., 1982) have shown that these limbic structures form two related anatomical units. The first unit includes the hippocampal formation with projections to the septal region and the mammillary bodies of the hypothalamus (fornix), then from the mammillary bodies to the anterior nuclei of the thalamus (the mammillothalamic tract), followed by projections to the cingulate and retrosplenial cortices (anterior limb of the internal capsule), and finally returning to the hippocampus (cingulum)—the so called Papez circuit (Papez, 1995). The second unit involves the amygdala and projections to the septal nuclei and dorsomedial nucleus of the thalamus (ventral amygdalofugal pathway), and from the thalamus to the prefrontal cortex (thalamocortical projections) and returning to the amygdala via the uncinate fasciculus (see Fig. 1).

Insight about the neuroanatomy of human memory has come from many sources, and this knowledge came without study of patients with stroke. The role of the hippocampus was demonstrated by epilepsy surgery (Scoville & Milner, 1957) and expanded and focused by anoxic brain injury (Zola-Morgan, Squire, & Amaral, 1986) and encephalitis (Cermak & O’Connor, 1983). A role of the diencephalon was demonstrated by thiamine deficiency particularly in alcoholics (Victor, Adams, & Collins, 1971) and extended by penetrating brain injury (Squire, Amaral, Zola-Morgan, Kritchevsky, & Press, 1989). These injuries have been considered relatively pure amnesia, but it is a very rare patient with any of these disorders or injuries who has purely amnesia. Only by compiling cases from disparate causes, with different associated deficits and with different lesion overlaps has core knowledge of the neuropsychology of amnesia emerged. Stroke has contributed to this knowledge. We will argue that a similar compilation of cases from diverse causes informs our understanding of the effects of non-limbic lesions on memory, and stroke has contributed to this understanding as well.

There are many reasons that stroke is often considered the optimal experiment of nature for unraveling the specific regional effects of brain disease on cognition. There are usually no pre-illness neurological issues to complicate interpretation of the effects of the index stroke (as in alcoholic Korsakoff's disease). With abrupt onset there is no concern about adaptive changes in the brain before the initial assessment (as in chronic epilepsy). Given time for accommodation and compensation of less specific acute effects of injury, post-acute impairments can be confidently attributed to the structural injury, and stroke generates relatively discrete lesion boundaries for mapping regional effects minimizing concerns about co-occurring diffuse, unmappable injuries (as in anoxia or encephalitis).

There are some limitations to reliance on stroke. Patients with stroke are often elderly and the effects of stroke need not be the same across the lifespan, nor can the effects of aging or undiagnosed degenerative diseases of aging always by eliminated. Whatever their age, patients with a clinical stroke often have accumulated years of subclinical vascular injury that can never be entirely discounted by imaging. Infarcts are constrained by the common patterns of vascular supply to the brain (see Fig. 2), and only a portion of the combinations of lesions that might be informative can be expected to emerge from a predictable vascular system. Hemorrhages are constrained by the locations of vessels that are predisposed to rupture and by planes of dissection. Although hemorrhages generate a greater range of lesion patterns, they are also associated with a greater amount of indirect, diffuse injury. Because neither respects functional anatomy, memory deficits from strokes frequently co-occur with language, visuospatial, and/or executive deficits. Finally, precise localization following a stroke is never incontrovertible; diaschisis can confuse lesion effects in the acute phase and plasticity and functional reorganization can obscure lesion effects in the later phases.

Between 20% and 50% of patients who survive a stroke complain about memory difficulties (Nys et al., 2005, Rasquin et al., 2002, Sorensen et al., 1982, Wade et al., 1986). There may be many specific causes, and not all of them directly related to the stroke: depression, medication effects, sleep disorders. We began our literature review with the assumption that there are two broad, directly stroke-related damage mechanisms. The medial location, complex connectivity and considerable bilateral integration of pathways make the limbic circuitry somewhat impervious to vascular injuries, but as a first mechanism, infarction or hemorrhage may involve any structure within the hippocampal or amygdala circuits (amnesia lesions) (Table 1). The second mechanism would be stroke outside of limbic circuitry impairing perceptual, cognitive, attentional or executive capacities essential for some aspect of memory (process lesions) (Table 2). Despite some limitations, stroke is the only etiology with the potential to inform and compare the effects of injury to every structure relevant to memory or learning, to demonstrate every critical pathway between those structures and to highlight the role non-limbic brain regions play in memory.

Section snippets

Methods

We searched the Medline database from 1966 to 2008 by combining the search terms “memory” or “amnesia” with the terms “stroke”, “hemorrhage”, or “ischemia”. In addition, we performed a second search by combining the search terms “posterior cerebral artery”, “thalamus”, “fornix”, “anterior communicating artery aneurysm”, or “cingulate” with the terms “amnesia”, or “memory”. English language article titles and abstracts when available were screened, and from this list, an initial series of papers

Strokes causing amnesia

For all examples in this category, there is a strong modality effect in humans that reflects the laterality of unilateral injury to limbic circuitry. Whatever the cause, including stroke, damage to left limbic structures produces impaired verbal memory (story recall, list learning, etc.) and some degree of visual memory (figures, designs, etc.) (Frisk & Milner, 1990). Damage to right limbic structures produces impaired configurational visual memory and perhaps impaired spatial memory (Doyon &

Conclusion

Review of the available literature on memory deficits after stroke reveals a few rigorous investigations of clinical–anatomical correlations (Alexander and Freedman, 1984, De Renzi et al., 1987, Graff-Radford et al., 1985, Graff-Radford et al., 1990, Van der Werf et al., 2003, Von Cramon et al., 1985, Von Cramon et al., 1988) and a large number of case reports focused on specific anatomical features. Although the original descriptions of clinical–anatomical correlations of amnesia did not

References (183)

  • J. DeLuca et al.

    Confabulation following aneurysm of the anterior communicating artery

    Cortex

    (1991)
  • J. Doyon et al.

    Right temporal-lobe contribution to global visual processing

    Neuropsychologia

    (1991)
  • P.J. Eslinger

    Altered serial position learning after frontal lobe lesions

    Neuropsychologia

    (1994)
  • V. Frisk et al.

    The role of the left hippocampal region in the acquisition and retention of story content

    Neuropsychologia

    (1990)
  • A. Gade et al.

    Temporal gradient in the remote memory impairment of amnesic patients with lesions in the basal forebrain

    Neuropsychologia

    (1990)
  • F.B. Gershberg et al.

    Impaired use of organizational strategies in free recall following frontal lobe damage

    Neuropsychologia

    (1995)
  • M. Habib et al.

    Pure topographical disorientation: A definition and anatomical basis

    Cortex

    (1987)
  • J.S. Janowsky et al.

    Source memory impairment in patients with frontal lobe lesions

    Neuropsychologia

    (1989)
  • W. Jetter et al.

    A verbal long term memory deficit in frontal lobe damaged patients

    Cortex

    (1986)
  • E. Jolkkonen et al.

    Projections from the amygdaloid complex to the magnocellular cholinergic basal forebrain in rat

    Neuroscience

    (2002)
  • D. Laplane et al.

    Bilateral infarction of the anterior cingulate gyri and of the fornices. Report of a case

    Journal of the Neurological Sciences

    (1981)
  • S. Luzzi et al.

    Topographical disorientation consequent to amnesia of spatial location in a patient with right parahippocampal damage

    Cortex

    (2000)
  • L.A. Miller et al.

    Right medial thalamic lesion causes isolated retrograde amnesia

    Neuropsychologia

    (2001)
  • B. Milner et al.

    Frontal-lobe contribution to recency judgements

    Neuropsychologia

    (1991)
  • K. Abe et al.

    Amnesia after a discrete basal forebrain lesion

    Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry

    (1998)
  • J.P. Aggleton et al.

    A comparison between the connections of the amygdala and hippocampus with the basal forebrain in the macaque

    Experimental Brain Research

    (1987)
  • J.P. Aggleton et al.

    Projections of the amygdala to the thalamus in the cynomolgus monkey

    Journal of Comparative Neurology

    (1984)
  • M.P. Alexander et al.

    Amnesia after anterior communicating artery aneurysm rupture

    Neurology

    (1984)
  • M.P. Alexander et al.

    Distributed anatomy of transcortical sensory aphasia

    Archives of Neurology

    (1989)
  • M.P. Alexander et al.

    Disorders of frontal lobe functioning

    Seminars in Neurology

    (2000)
  • M.P. Alexander et al.

    California Verbal Learning Test: Performance by patients with focal frontal and non-frontal lesions

    Brain

    (2003)
  • C.R. Archer et al.

    Case report. Aphasia in thalamic stroke: CT stereotactic localization

    Journal of Computer Assisted Tomography

    (1981)
  • S. Becker et al.

    A computational model of prefrontal control in free recall: Strategic memory use in the California Verbal Learning Task

    Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

    (2003)
  • C. Bellebaum et al.

    Clipping versus coiling: Neuropsychological follow up after aneurysmal subarachnoid haemorrhage (SAH)

    Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology

    (2004)
  • D.F. Benson et al.

    The amnestic syndrome of posterior cerebral artery occlusion

    Acta Neurologica Scandinavica

    (1974)
  • N. Berlyne

    Confabulation

    British Journal of Psychiatry

    (1972)
  • C.M. Bird et al.

    Visual neglect after right posterior cerebral artery infarction

    Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry

    (2006)
  • J. Bogousslavsky et al.

    The syndrome of unilateral tuberothalamic artery territory infarction

    Stroke

    (1986)
  • J. Bogousslavsky et al.

    Thalamic infarcts: Clinical syndromes, etiology, and prognosis

    Neurology

    (1988)
  • S. Bottger et al.

    Neurobehavioural disturbances, rehabilitation outcome, and lesion site in patients after rupture and repair of anterior communicating artery aneurysm

    Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry

    (1998)
  • W.R. Brain

    Visual disorientation with special reference to lesions of the right cerebral hemisphere

    Brain

    (1941)
  • P.W. Burgess et al.

    Function and localization within rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10)

    Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences

    (2007)
  • N. Cals et al.

    Pure superficial posterior cerebral artery territory infarction in The Lausanne Stroke Registry

    Journal of Neurology

    (2002)
  • L.R. Caplan

    “Top of the basilar” syndrome

    Neurology

    (1980)
  • L.R. Caplan et al.

    Cuing and memory dysfunction in alexia without agraphia

    Brain

    (1974)
  • E. Carrera et al.

    Anteromedian, central, and posterolateral infarcts of the thalamus: Three variant types

    Stroke

    (2004)
  • P. Castaigne et al.

    Paramedian thalamic and midbrain infarct: Clinical and neuropathological study

    Annals of Neurology

    (1981)
  • C. Chayer et al.

    Frontal lobe functions

    Current Neurology and Neuroscience Reports

    (2001)
  • S. Clarke et al.

    Pure amnesia after unilateral left polar thalamic infarct: Topographic and sequential neuropsychological and metabolic (PET) correlations, Journal of Neurology

    Neurosurgery and Psychiatry

    (1994)
  • W. Cremonini et al.

    Contrasting performance of right- and left-hemisphere patients on short-term and long-term sequential visual memory

    Neuropsychologia

    (1980)
  • Cited by (43)

    • Rodent models used in preclinical studies of deep brain stimulation to rescue memory deficits

      2021, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
      Citation Excerpt :

      Many studies have linked white matter lesions and subcortical damage to cognitive impairment in patients with vascular dementia (Reed et al., 2004; Prins et al., 2004). Landmark neuropsychological studies have reported deficits in episodic memory after stroke or ischemia which have been linked to the selective damage of limbic structures (Lim and Alexander, 2009). Interestingly, memory disorders can occur when the hippocampus and mesiotemporal structures have been spared by ischemia.

    • Major neurocognitive disorder followıng isolated hippocampal ischemıc lesions

      2017, Journal of the Neurological Sciences
      Citation Excerpt :

      It has been showed that hippocampus is working as time dependent manner and related to conscious recollection of the learned items. Neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that hippocampus activity is related both encoding as well as recollection processes, and showed that regions dentate gyrus and CA fields 2 and 3 were active during episodic encoding and that the subiculum was more active during the recollection of the learning episode [24–26]. Verbal and visual delayed memory as well as story recall were disturbed in all patients.

    • Complex assessment of distinct cognitive impairments following ouabain injection into the rat dorsoloateral striatum

      2015, Behavioural Brain Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      The striatum was shown to be involved in episodic encoding [26,27]. In humans, stroke may induce episodic memory disorders, disturbing the quality of everyday life [3]. In rodents, episodic-like memory could be assessed using tasks based on spontaneous novelty preference and objects, as well as their location recognition [28,29].

    • Amnesia: General

      2015, International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences: Second Edition
    • Hypoxia/ischemia a key player in early post stroke seizures: Modulation by opioidergic and nitrergic systems

      2015, European Journal of Pharmacology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Thus, the decrease in PTZ induced seizure threshold might be a consequence of H/I induced neuronal death and associated morphological and physiological changes that are only partly elucidated. Stroke like other neurogolical conditions leads to several morphological and physiological changes, causing subsequent complications such as paralysis, memory impairement, and seizures (Lim and Alexander, 2009; Ring and Weingarden, 2007). Indeed, there are evolving evidences suggesting that neural damages regadless of their etiologies can cause seizures (Haas et al., 2001).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text