Skip to main content
Advertisement

Main menu

  • Home
  • Content
    • Current Issue
    • Accepted Manuscripts
    • Article Preview
    • Past Issue Archive
    • Video Articles
    • AJNR Case Collection
    • Case of the Week Archive
    • Case of the Month Archive
    • Classic Case Archive
  • Special Collections
    • AJNR Awards
    • Low-Field MRI
    • Alzheimer Disease
    • ASNR Foundation Special Collection
    • Photon-Counting CT
    • View All
  • Multimedia
    • AJNR Podcasts
    • AJNR SCANtastic
    • Trainee Corner
    • MRI Safety Corner
    • Imaging Protocols
  • For Authors
    • Submit a Manuscript
    • Submit a Video Article
    • Submit an eLetter to the Editor/Response
    • Manuscript Submission Guidelines
    • Statistical Tips
    • Fast Publishing of Accepted Manuscripts
    • Graphical Abstract Preparation
    • Imaging Protocol Submission
    • Author Policies
  • About Us
    • About AJNR
    • Editorial Board
    • Editorial Board Alumni
  • More
    • Become a Reviewer/Academy of Reviewers
    • Subscribers
    • Permissions
    • Alerts
    • Feedback
    • Advertisers
    • ASNR Home

User menu

  • Alerts
  • Log in

Search

  • Advanced search
American Journal of Neuroradiology
American Journal of Neuroradiology

American Journal of Neuroradiology

ASHNR American Society of Functional Neuroradiology ASHNR American Society of Pediatric Neuroradiology ASSR
  • Alerts
  • Log in

Advanced Search

  • Home
  • Content
    • Current Issue
    • Accepted Manuscripts
    • Article Preview
    • Past Issue Archive
    • Video Articles
    • AJNR Case Collection
    • Case of the Week Archive
    • Case of the Month Archive
    • Classic Case Archive
  • Special Collections
    • AJNR Awards
    • Low-Field MRI
    • Alzheimer Disease
    • ASNR Foundation Special Collection
    • Photon-Counting CT
    • View All
  • Multimedia
    • AJNR Podcasts
    • AJNR SCANtastic
    • Trainee Corner
    • MRI Safety Corner
    • Imaging Protocols
  • For Authors
    • Submit a Manuscript
    • Submit a Video Article
    • Submit an eLetter to the Editor/Response
    • Manuscript Submission Guidelines
    • Statistical Tips
    • Fast Publishing of Accepted Manuscripts
    • Graphical Abstract Preparation
    • Imaging Protocol Submission
    • Author Policies
  • About Us
    • About AJNR
    • Editorial Board
    • Editorial Board Alumni
  • More
    • Become a Reviewer/Academy of Reviewers
    • Subscribers
    • Permissions
    • Alerts
    • Feedback
    • Advertisers
    • ASNR Home
  • Follow AJNR on Twitter
  • Visit AJNR on Facebook
  • Follow AJNR on Instagram
  • Join AJNR on LinkedIn
  • RSS Feeds

AJNR Awards, New Junior Editors, and more. Read the latest AJNR updates

Research ArticlePediatric Neuroimaging

Dyslexic Children Have Abnormal Brain Lactate Response to Reading-Related Language Tasks

Todd L. Richards, Stephen R. Dager, David Corina, Sandra Serafini, Aaron C. Heide, Keith Steury, Wayne Strauss, Cecil E. Hayes, Robert D. Abbott, Suzanne Craft, Dennis Shaw, Stefan Posse and Virginia W. Berninger
American Journal of Neuroradiology September 1999, 20 (8) 1393-1398;
Todd L. Richards
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Stephen R. Dager
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
David Corina
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Sandra Serafini
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Aaron C. Heide
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Keith Steury
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Wayne Strauss
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Cecil E. Hayes
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Robert D. Abbott
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Suzanne Craft
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Dennis Shaw
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Stefan Posse
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Virginia W. Berninger
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • Responses
  • References
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

BACKGROUND AND PURPOSE: Children with dyslexia have difficulty learning to recognize written words owing to subtle deficits in oral language related to processing sounds and accessing words automatically. The purpose of this study was to compare regional changes in brain lactate between dyslexic children and control subjects during oral language activation.

METHODS: Brain lactate metabolism was measured during four different cognitive tasks (three language tasks and one nonlanguage task) in six dyslexic boys and in seven control subjects (age- and IQ-matched right-handed boys who are good readers) using a fast MR spectroscopic imaging technique called proton echo-planar spectroscopic imaging (1-cm3 voxel resolution). The area under the N-acetylaspartate (NAA) and lactate peaks was measured to calculate the lactate/NAA ratio in each voxel.

RESULTS: Dyslexic boys showed a greater area of brain lactate elevation (2.33 ± SE 0.843 voxels) as compared with the control group (0.57 ± SE 0.30 voxels) during a phonological task in the left anterior quadrant. No significant differences were observed in the nonlanguage tasks.

CONCLUSION: Dyslexic and control children differ in brain lactate metabolism when performing language tasks, but do not differ in nonlanguage auditory tasks.

Dyslexia, or specific reading disability, is the most frequently occurring learning disability and the most common disorder of childhood. Estimated to affect 5% to 15% of children, dyslexia is characterized as unexpected underachievement in reading for one's intellectual ability. It has been well-established that it is a language-based disorder, often caused by deficits in phonological processing (1–4). Although dyslexia is a genetic disorder (5), with reported linkage to chromosomes 6 and 15 (6, 7), its phenotypic expression (V. Berninger, et al, unpublished data, 1999) depends on the environment as well as heredity (8, 9). Twenty years of behavioral evidence support two major causal mechanisms in dyslexia: deficient phonological processing of spoken words (1–4) and inability to visually access names in the lexicon (mental dictionary) automatically (10, 11). The consequence of these deficiencies is poor reading, which differs from good reading in verbal efficiency (4). Several investigators have found associations between neurophysiological abnormalities and dyslexia (12–16). Functional neuroimaging studies with 18F-fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography (FDG-PET) indicate that adult dyslexics have focal increases in glucose metabolism (12, 13), suggesting either inefficient processing or the activation of compensatory pathways (14).

Based on what is known about the metabolic role of lactate and glucose during brain activation, we hypothesized that brain functional inefficiencies would exist in dyslexics, specifically manifested as elevated lactate covering a greater area of the brain during language. Lactate is known to be metabolized in the brain as a neuronal substrate (17) and also as a by-product for glucose metabolism during brain activation. MR spectroscopy has previously been used to demonstrate lactate activation (increase in lactate) in healthy adults during visual, auditory, and cognitive tasks (18–22). In these studies, lactate was observed to increase rapidly during sensory stimulation in a regionally specific manner.

We used a novel noninvasive technique called proton echo-planar spectroscopic imaging (PEPSI) (23) to investigate metabolic brain activation during oral language tasks in dyslexic and control children. Previous neuroimaging studies of language have used either PET (24) or functional MR imaging (25), which uses blood oxygenation level–dependent image contrast changes to map out regional brain activation (an indirect measure of metabolism). Functional MR spectroscopy performed with the PEPSI technique is an alternative approach for detecting regional brain activation and measuring tissue-based lactate changes (a direct measure of metabolism) produced by a temporary mismatch of oxygen delivery and consumption in response to neuronal activation (26). For this study, we specifically tested the hypothesis that a greater area of brain activation (higher voxel counts) would occur with language processing in dyslexic children than in control subjects.

Methods

Study Design

Six dyslexic and seven nondyslexic (control) boys were imaged using the PEPSI technique (23) while they performed four different cognitive tasks. The dyslexic and control groups were well-matched in age, IQ, and head size (number of total voxels) but not in reading skills, in which they demonstrated marked differences, as described below. The experimental tasks were designed to activate phonological and lexical access functions of the brain while a tone task was used to activate auditory nonlanguage functions of the brain. Scanner noise and passive listening (to word lists used in both the phonological and lexical access tasks) were two control tasks used to subtract out low-level acoustic and nonspecific stimulus effects, respectively. The phonological and lexical access tasks engage additional linguistic processes beyond those required for passive listening of language. The unique brain activation evoked by processing rhymes or accessing word meanings, independent of brain activation due to the characteristics of word stimuli, was assessed by subtracting out the passive listening condition from the phonological and lexical access tasks. The component of brain activation related to processing requirements for auditory functions not specific to language was assessed by subtracting out the scanner noise from the tone judgments.

MR Imaging and Spectroscopy

Conventional MR imaging and PEPSI were performed on a clinical 1.5-T Signa MR imaging system from General Electric equipped with version 5.4 software and a custom-built radiofrequency coil developed by Hayes et al (27). MR images were acquired in the sagittal plane (600/20 [TR/TE]) and also in the axial plane (2000/35,80). The custom-designed coil was necessary to acquire MR spectroscopic data with high enough signal-to-noise ratio to detect the small lactate peak. The coordinates of the sylvian fissure and surrounding language-related structures were determined from the sagittal and axial images and used to determine the axial section for spectroscopic imaging. The areas sampled with PEPSI were based on the work of Ojemann et al (28), which invasively demonstrated language activation in the anatomic region encompassing the sylvian fissure and adjacent opercula. Deeper subcortical structures were also included that are associated (through neuronal connectivity) with the cortical areas. Proton spectra were acquired using PEPSI, a spin-echo pulse sequence developed by Posse et al (23) that allows fast spectroscopic imaging that is 32 times faster than conventional hydrogen spectroscopic imaging for the same spatial resolution. Parameters for data acquisition were as follows: 4000/272/2, 32 × 16 spatial matrix, 512 echoes in the echo-planar acquisition, 32 complex points per echo, full echo acquisition, 24-cm field of view, and 20-mm section thickness. Spatial resolution was approximately 1 cm3. Data were processed as described previously (22). The metabolites were integrated using the following procedure: 1) magnetic field inhomogeneity (B0) shifts were corrected by finding the maximum point of the NAA peak and resetting the ppm scale to 2.0 ppm for each spectrum; 2) the average baseline was determined from 32 points to the right of 0.0 ppm; 3) the maximum intensity point of the peak was determined within a set spectral range (NAA = 2.0 ± 0.07, lactate = 1.3 ± 0.1 ppm); and 4) integration was performed by summing the spectral intensities for the NAA and lactate for the ppm ranges specified in step 3.

Subject Characterization

The University of Washington Human Subjects Institutional Review Board approved this study, and each subject (as well as parent/guardian) gave written, informed consent. All subjects were right handed (90% to 100% on the Edinburgh handedness scale) (29). The control subjects had a history of learning to read easily and were reading above normal for age (average was 1 SD above mean for age using the Woodcock Reading Mastery Test-Revised [WRMT-R]) (30). The dyslexic boys had a developmental history of extreme difficulty in learning to read despite many forms of extra assistance at school, and also had a family history of multigenerational dyslexia, which was confirmed in a concurrent family genetics study (W. Raskind, personal communication) at our center. The dyslexic boys were reading on average 1.66 SD below the mean for age using the WRMT-R test (30). In addition, all the dyslexic boys were shown to have a triple deficit in three skills that predict ease of learning to read and response to intervention, phonological (phoneme segmentation and/or memory for spoken nonwords), rapid automatized naming, and orthographic (speed of coding written words and/or accuracy of representing them in memory) (31). Based on independent t-tests, the seven control subjects (mean = 127.3, SD = 10.8) and six dyslexic boys (mean = 124.3, SD = 11.1) did not differ in age in months: t(11) = 0.49, P = .637. Likewise, the control subjects (mean = 15.6, SD = 3.2) and dyslexic children (mean = 13.2, SD = 1.6) did not differ in age-corrected WISC-III vocabulary scores (t[11] = 1.68, P = .12), which provide the best estimate of full-scale IQ. However, the control and dyslexic boys did differ significantly in age-corrected standard scores for reading real words on the word identification (WI) subtest of the WRMT-R test and for reading pseudowords on the Word Attack (WA) subtest of the WRMT-R (t[11] = 6.81, P < .001 on the WI subtest and t[10] = 6.02, P < .001 on the WA subtest). The differences for both real-word reading (WI, control subjects: mean = 115.1, SD = 9.2; dyslexics: mean = 75.5, SD = 11.8) and pseudoword reading (WA, control subjects: mean = 110.2, SD = 6.8; dyslexics: mean = 79.0, SD = 10.7) were large as well as statistically significant.

Language Tasks

During MR imaging, the children were asked to listen to aurally presented words, nonwords, and tone pairs at a rate of one stimulus pair every 4 seconds. Language stimuli were composed of four groupings of word pairs, crossed for lexical status (word vs nonword) and sound similarity (rhyming vs nonrhyming), resulting in four sets of stimuli: word/word:nonrhyming (eg, FLY-CHURCH); word/word:rhyming (FLY-EYE), word/nonword:nonrhyming (CROW-TREEL); word/nonword:rhyming (MEAL-TREEL). Nonwords, such as TREEL, allow assessment of sound processing without any meaning cues. The presenting order of word pair-types were counterbalanced and thus the ordering effects were controlled for. During rhyming (phonological task), subjects listened to the same stimulus material and judged whether the word pairs rhymed or did not rhyme; whether words were real was irrelevant. During the lexical access condition, subjects listened to the word pairs and judged whether the word pairs contained two real words or contained a nonword; whether the words rhymed was irrelevant. Thus, the same stimulus lists were used for lexical access and rhyming, only the task instructions changed. Subjects indicated their rhyme and lexical decisions by raising cards held in the right and left hands (the hand used to signal a Yes response was counterbalanced across subjects). During passive listening, subjects listened to the same stimuli but were instructed to alternately raise the left and right hand without making any judgments on the stimuli. For the tone judgment task, five pure tones (329.6 Hz, 350.0 Hz, 415.3 Hz, 440.0 Hz, and 523.0 Hz) were grouped into pairs of identical tones or different tones. Subjects were asked to raise one hand if the tones were identical and to raise the opposite hand if the tones were different. The subjects were tested for accuracy of their responses for all tasks during a prescan training session and during the actual MR examination. For the tone subtraction, the baseline scanner noise was used instead of passive listening. A recovery period of 5 minutes between tasks was based in part on the lactate recovery measurements by Frahm et al (26).

Data Analysis

To affirmatively evaluate focal brain activation, z-score maps were created from the lactate/NAA ratios based on the following equation: Math where (task) refers to the task given during the MR examination: phonological, lexical access, or tone differentiation. The SD of the lactate/NAA was calculated for each subject using all valid spectra of the control task (either passive listening or scanner noise). This z-score was calculated for all voxels that contained valid spectra for each language condition (22). Definition of lactate elevation was based on z-scores greater than 2.0 on a voxel-by-voxel basis. The number of voxels with elevated lactate within each quadrant was counted for each subject.

The PEPSI data were analyzed to ascertain the number of voxels (computerized volume elements) with elevated lactate in four regions of the brain. Because regional specificity of lactate response is not well established and also because of the large variability among subjects in the spatial location of the lactate response, we divided the spectroscopic imaging section into four quadrants. The brain was divided into four quadrants based on left to right (brain midline defined on the axial MR image) and anterior to posterior (using the midpoint of the thalamus as a landmark). Inferential statistics were used to compare relative activation for each group in each brain quadrant on each task. ANOVA was used to test for differences in the number of activated voxels between control and dyslexic subjects. The number of valid voxels for the dyslexic group was not significantly different from that of the control group (controls: mean = 160.6, SD = 6.9; dyslexics: mean = 166, SD = 19; t[11] = 0.76, P < .48).

The lactate/NAA ratio was used to normalize for radiofrequency inhomogeneity, variable CSF contribution, and also to standardize the lactate signal across subjects. An automated computer-software mask was applied to the spectra to ensure that the MR lactate signal was not contaminated with scalp lipid signal (22). A true lactate peak was differentiated from lipid contamination on the basis of MR frequency, which was determined from an in vitro lactate measurement (the lipid peak MR frequency was not the same as lactate frequency). Additionally, in previous work, we varied the TE to assess J-coupling properties of the peak at 1.3 ppm, which identified this peak as lactate (32).

Results

Dyslexic boys had significantly more brain voxels with elevated MR lactate levels (2.33 ± SE 0.843) than did the control group (0.57 ± SE 0.30) during a phonological task in the left anterior quadrant (ANOVA: F[1,11] = 4.41, P = .05, see Figs 1 and 2). None of the other quadrants were significantly different between dyslexic and control subjects for the other three quadrants during the phonological task (P values ranged from .62 to .74), during the lexical access task (P values ranged from .37 to .96), during the tone task (P values ranged from .32 to .92), or during the scanner noise alone (P values ranged from .23 to .99). The fact that there was no difference between the dyslexic and control subjects during the tone task implies that the difference between dyslexics and control subjects is specific to auditory language function and not to nonlinguistic auditory function. Behavioral data during scan sessions demonstrated that all subjects performed the tasks above chance levels, showing attentiveness to the task, and that the dyslexics had a significantly lower correct response rate in responding to phonological tasks than did the control group (dyslexics: mean = 89.6, SD = 3.9; controls: mean = 96.7, SD = 2.9; t[10] = 3.78, P < .004). Behavioral differences between the dyslexics and control subjects during the tone task, however, were not statistically significant (t[10] = 1.7, P = .12).

fig 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
fig 1.

Bar graphs show number of activated voxels (as defined by MR spectroscopic lactate increases) in the four quadrants of the brain for both dyslexic and control children.

A–C, Graphs of phonological task data (A), lexical access task (B), and tone task (C). (Error bars are standard error of the mean; asterisk indicates dyslexic versus control comparisons that were significantly different).

Discussion

Our main finding was that the dyslexic children in comparison with nonimpaired children had a larger regional distribution of metabolic activation, characterized by the number of voxels with elevated lactate, in the left anterior quadrant during the phonological task. This greater regional activation cannot be directly attributed to the dyslexic children's reading difficulty, because the tasks were auditory and did not involve reading per se. Concurrently collected behavioral data verified that the subjects were attentively listening to the auditory stimuli and also that the dyslexic children were less accurate than the control group on the phonological task. Our findings suggest that the dyslexic brain has a larger regional distribution of metabolic activation (as evidenced by the increased number of voxels with elevated lactate) than normal brain during the mental process of accomplishing the same linguistic task (although the dyslexic children made more linguistic errors). This metabolic abnormality points toward a phonological processing problem located in the left anterior quadrant (this quadrant includes the left frontal lobe and language areas surrounding the sylvian fissure). This increased metabolic energy demand may be the neurologic substrate underlying the behavioral phenomenon described by the verbal efficiency theory (4). According to this theory, poor readers must exert more mental effort (because of inefficient language processing) than good readers to accomplish the same task. Further work will be required to determine whether the greater activation reflects inefficient processing within language areas or the activation of compensatory pathways (ie, a functional brain pathway outside the normal brain activation region that is established to compensate for dyslexic deficits).

Increases in the MR lactate signal have been observed in the occipital cortex during photic stimulation (19, 20) in the auditory cortex during 1-kHz tone pulses (33), in the basal ganglia during finger movements (34), and in the limbic region of the brain during seizure activity (35). The increase in lactate is thought to be related to an increase in glucose metabolism during neuronal stimulation.

Recent studies using functional MR imaging by Shaywitz et al (15) have also shown that adult dyslexic subjects exhibit different brain activation patterns than nonimpaired subjects during a phonological language task as compared with an orthographic coding task. These investigators found significant overactivation in the left inferior frontal gyrus (see Fig 2 in [15]) in the dyslexic subjects as compared with control subjects (15). These findings for adults are consistent (although more anatomically localized) with our findings for children, inasmuch as we also found regional metabolic overactivation relative to control subjects in the left anterior quadrant, which included the left inferior frontal gyrus. Using FDG-PET, Hagman et al (12) found higher metabolism in the medial temporal lobe bilaterally in adult dyslexics during an auditory syllable discrimination task. FDG and lactate are both involved in carbohydrate metabolism of the brain and both are involved in energy metabolism during neuronal activation. Rumsey et al (36), using 15O PET to measure blood flow, found that the left inferior frontal cortex is involved in phonological processing. One difference between our results and those in the studies mentioned above is that the pattern of metabolic activation that we observed with lactate included subcortical areas as well as cortical areas (cortical activation areas are reported by the PET and functional MR imaging studies). The subcortical activation areas that we observed may reflect neuronal connectivity from the adjacent cortical areas involved with language.

fig 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
fig 2.

MR image and proton spectra from an activated brain region of one dyslexic subject.

A, MR image with white box indicating the brain region measured with MR spectroscopy.

B, Proton MR spectrum from the white box brain region during the passive listening task.

C, Proton MR spectrum from the white box brain region during the phonological task. The intensity axis of the spectra is scaled so that the lactate can be seen easier; however, choline (Cho) and NAA are scaled off the figure. Note the increase in the lactate peaks during the phonological task as compared with passive listening.

Our findings are important because they shed new light on brain mechanisms involved with dyslexia at a developmental stage when dyslexia is amenable to treatment (8, 37). Because most children respond positively to instructional interventions, many educators have rejected the notion that reading problems are brain-based. Functional brain differences between dyslexics and control subjects such as those demonstrated by this study add evidence that dyslexia is a brain-based disorder. We have shown that there is a difference in the regional distribution of lactate elevation between dyslexics and control subjects; however, it is premature to use this technique as a diagnostic tool for dyslexia.

Conclusion

We have established differences in the regional distribution of brain lactate metabolism between dyslexic and control children (matched for age, IQ, and head size) during an aurally presented phonological task. Our study extends previous work that found differences in patterns of cerebral blood flow between adult dyslexics and control subjects in visually presented functional MR imaging reading tasks (15) and in phonological and orthographic tasks during PET scanning (17).

Acknowledgments

We gratefully acknowledge contributions from Darrell Hochman and Martin Kuschmerick to this manuscript, and editorial assistance from Jane Johnson.

Footnotes

  • ↵3 Supported by a Special Multidisciplinary Learning Disabilities Center grant from NIH (NICHD), P50 HD33812.

    Presented in part at the annual meeting of the Cognitive Neuroscience Society, San Francisco, CA, April 1998.

    Address reprint requests to Todd L. Richards, PhD, Department of Radiology, Box 357115, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195.

References

  1. ↵
    Liberman I, Shankweiler D, Fisher F, Carter B. Explicit syllable and phoneme segmentation in the young child. . J Exp Child Psychol 1974;18:201-212
    CrossRef
  2. Bradley L, Bryant P. Categorizing sounds and learning to read: a causal connection. . Nature 1983;301:419-421
    CrossRef
  3. Wagner R, Toregsen J. The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. . Psychol Bull 1987;101:192-212
    CrossRef
  4. ↵
    Perfetti C. Reading Ability. . New York: Oxford University Press; 1985;
  5. ↵
    DeFries J, Fulker D, LaBuda M. Evidence for a genetic aetiology in reading disability of twins. . Nature 1987;329:537-539
    CrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    Cardon L, Smith S, Fulker D, Kimberling W, Pennington B, DeFries J. Quantitative trait locus for reading disability on chromosome 6. . Science 1994;266:276-279
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  7. Grigorenko E, Wood F, Meyer M,, et al. Susceptibility loci for distinct components of developmental dyslexia and chromosomes 6 and 15. . Am J Hum Genet 1997;60:27-39
    CrossRefPubMed
  8. Berninger V. Reading and Writing Acquisition: A Developmental Neuropsychological Perspective. . Madison, WI: Brown & Benchmark; 1994;
  9. Olson R, Forsberg H, Wise B. Genes, environment, and the development of orthographic skills. . In: Berninger VW, ed. The Varieties of Orthographic Knowledge, I: Theoretical and Developmental Issues. Dordrcht, the Netherlands: Kluwer; 1994;27-71
  10. ↵
    Denckla M, Rudel R. Rapid “automatized” naming (R.A.N.): dyslexia differentiated from other learning disabilities. . Neuropsychologia 1976;14:471-479
    CrossRefPubMed
  11. Wolf M, Bally H, Morris R. Automaticity, retrieval processes, and reading: a longitudinal study in average and impaired reading. . Child Dev 1986;57:988-1000
    CrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    Hagman JO, Wood F, Buchsbaum MS, Tallal P, Flowers L, Katz W. Cerebral brain metabolism in adult dyslexic subjects assessed with positron emission tomography during performance of an auditory task. . Arch Neurol 1992;49:734-739
    CrossRefPubMed
  13. Gross-Glenn K, Duara R, Barker WW,, et al. Positron emission tomographic studies during serial word-reading by normal and dyslexic adults. . J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1991;13:531-544
    PubMed
  14. ↵
    Rumsey JM. Neuroimaging in developmental dyslexia. . In: Lyon GR, Rumsey JM, eds. Neuroimaging: A Window to the Neurological Foundations of Learning and Behavior in Children. Baltimore: Brookes; 1996;57-77
  15. ↵
    Shaywitz SE, Shaywitz BA, Pugh KR,, et al. Functional disruption in the organization of the brain for reading in dyslexia. . Proc Natl Acad Sci 1998;95:2636-2641
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  16. Rumsey JM, Nace K, Donohue B, Wise D, Maisog JM, Andreason P. A positron emission tomographic study of impaired word recognition and phonological processing in dyslexic men. . Arch Neurol 1997;54:562-573
    CrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    Tsacopoulos M, Magistretti PJ. Metabolic coupling between glia and neurons. . J Neurosci 1996;16:877-885
    FREE Full Text
  18. ↵
    Frahm J, Kruger G, Merboldt KD, Kleinschmidt A. Dynamic uncoupling and recoupling of perfusion and oxidative metabolism during focal brain activation in man. . Magn Reson Med 1996;35:143-148
    PubMed
  19. ↵
    Prichard J, Rothman D, Novotny E,, et al. Lactate rise detected by H-1 NMR in human visual cortex during physiologic stimulation. . Proc Natl Acad Sci 1992;88:5829-5831
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  20. Sappey-Marinier D, Calabrese G, Fein G, Hugg JW, Biggins C, Weiner MW. Effect of photic stimulation on human visual cortex lactate and phosphates using 1H and 31P MR spectroscopy. . J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 1992;12:584-592
    PubMed
  21. Richards TL, Gates GA, Gardner JC,, et al. Functional MR spectroscopy of the auditory cortex in healthy subjects and patients with sudden hearing loss. . AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 1997;18:611-620
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  22. ↵
    Richards TL, Dager SR, Panagiotides HS,, et al. Functional MR spectroscopy during language activation: a preliminary study using proton echo-planar spectroscopic imaging (PEPSI). . Int J Neuroradiol 1997;3:490-495
  23. ↵
    Posse S, Dager SR, Richards TL,, et al. In vivo measurement of regional brain metabolic response to hyperventilation using magnetic resonance proton echo planar spectroscopic imaging (PEPSI). . Magn Reson Med 1997;37:858-865
    PubMed
  24. ↵
    Eulitz C, Elbert T, Bartenstein P, Weiller C, Muller SP, Pantev C. Comparison of magnetic and metabolic brain activity during a verb generation task. . Neuroreport 1994;6:97-100
    PubMed
  25. ↵
    Binder JR, Frost JA, Hammeke TA, Cox RW, Rao SM, Prieto T. Human brain language areas identified by functional magnetic resonance imaging. . J Neurosci 1997;17:353-362
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  26. ↵
    Frahm J, Krueger G, Merboldt KD, Kleinschmidt A. Dynamic NMR studies of perfusion and oxidative metabolism during focal brain activation. . Adv Exp Med Biol 1997;413:195-203
    PubMed
  27. ↵
    Hayes CE, Mathis CM. Improved brain coil for fMRI and high resolution imaging. . Presented at the annual meeting of the Society of Magnetic Resonance, Berkeley, CA, April, 1996;
  28. ↵
    Ojemann G. Localization of language in frontal cortex. . Adv Neurol 1992;57:361-368
    PubMed
  29. ↵
    Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh Inventory. . Neuropsychologia 1971;9:97-113
    CrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    Woodcock R. Woodcock Reading Mastery Test-Revised. . Minneapolis, MN: American Guidance Service; 1987;
  31. ↵
    Berninger V. Process Assessment of the Learner: Guides for Intervention. . San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1998;
  32. ↵
    Dager SR, Marro KI, Peterson J, Richards TL. Applications of magnetic resonance spectroscopy to investigate panic disorder. . In: Nasrallah H, Pettegrew J, eds NMR Spectroscopy in Psychiatric Brain Disorders. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press; 1995;147-177
  33. ↵
    Singh M. Toward proton MR spectroscopic imaging of stimulated brain function. . IEEE Trans Nucl Sci 1992;39:1161-1164
    CrossRef
  34. ↵
    Kuwabara T, Watanabe H, Tsuji S, Yuasa T. Lactate rise in the basal ganglia accompanying finger movements: a localized 1H-MRS study. . Brain Res 1995;670:326-328
    CrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    Prichard JW. Nuclear magnetic resonance spectroscopy of seizure states. . Epilepsia 1994;35:S14-S20
  36. ↵
    Rumsey JM, Horwitz B, Donohue BC, Nace K, Maisog JM, Andreason P. Phonological and orthographic components of word recognition: a PET-rCBF study. . Brain 1997;120:739-759
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  37. Lovett M, Ransby M, Hardwick N, Johns M, Donaldson S. Can dyslexia be treated? Treatment specific and generalized treatment effects in dyslexic children's response to remediation. . Brain Lang 1989;37:90-121
    CrossRefPubMed
  • Received September 29, 1998.
  • Accepted after revision March 24, 1999.
  • Copyright © American Society of Neuroradiology
View Abstract
PreviousNext
Back to top

In this issue

American Journal of Neuroradiology
Vol. 20, Issue 8
1 Sep 1999
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
Advertisement
Print
Download PDF
Email Article

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on American Journal of Neuroradiology.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Dyslexic Children Have Abnormal Brain Lactate Response to Reading-Related Language Tasks
(Your Name) has sent you a message from American Journal of Neuroradiology
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the American Journal of Neuroradiology web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Cite this article
Todd L. Richards, Stephen R. Dager, David Corina, Sandra Serafini, Aaron C. Heide, Keith Steury, Wayne Strauss, Cecil E. Hayes, Robert D. Abbott, Suzanne Craft, Dennis Shaw, Stefan Posse, Virginia W. Berninger
Dyslexic Children Have Abnormal Brain Lactate Response to Reading-Related Language Tasks
American Journal of Neuroradiology Sep 1999, 20 (8) 1393-1398;

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
0 Responses
Respond to this article
Share
Bookmark this article
Dyslexic Children Have Abnormal Brain Lactate Response to Reading-Related Language Tasks
Todd L. Richards, Stephen R. Dager, David Corina, Sandra Serafini, Aaron C. Heide, Keith Steury, Wayne Strauss, Cecil E. Hayes, Robert D. Abbott, Suzanne Craft, Dennis Shaw, Stefan Posse, Virginia W. Berninger
American Journal of Neuroradiology Sep 1999, 20 (8) 1393-1398;
del.icio.us logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Conclusion
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • Responses
  • References
  • PDF

Related Articles

  • No related articles found.
  • PubMed
  • Google Scholar

Cited By...

  • Effects of a Phonologically Driven Treatment for Dyslexia on Lactate Levels Measured by Proton MR Spectroscopic Imaging
  • Crossref
  • Google Scholar

This article has not yet been cited by articles in journals that are participating in Crossref Cited-by Linking.

More in this TOC Section

  • FRACTURE MR in Congenital Vertebral Anomalies
  • Neuroimaging Delineation and Progression of SLSMD
  • fetal brain development of 10 weeks gestation
Show more PEDIATRIC NEUROIMAGING

Similar Articles

Advertisement

Indexed Content

  • Current Issue
  • Accepted Manuscripts
  • Article Preview
  • Past Issues
  • Editorials
  • Editor's Choice
  • Fellows' Journal Club
  • Letters to the Editor
  • Video Articles

Cases

  • Case Collection
  • Archive - Case of the Week
  • Archive - Case of the Month
  • Archive - Classic Case

More from AJNR

  • Trainee Corner
  • Imaging Protocols
  • MRI Safety Corner
  • Book Reviews

Multimedia

  • AJNR Podcasts
  • AJNR Scantastics

Resources

  • Turnaround Time
  • Submit a Manuscript
  • Submit a Video Article
  • Submit an eLetter to the Editor/Response
  • Manuscript Submission Guidelines
  • Statistical Tips
  • Fast Publishing of Accepted Manuscripts
  • Graphical Abstract Preparation
  • Imaging Protocol Submission
  • Evidence-Based Medicine Level Guide
  • Publishing Checklists
  • Author Policies
  • Become a Reviewer/Academy of Reviewers
  • News and Updates

About Us

  • About AJNR
  • Editorial Board
  • Editorial Board Alumni
  • Alerts
  • Permissions
  • Not an AJNR Subscriber? Join Now
  • Advertise with Us
  • Librarian Resources
  • Feedback
  • Terms and Conditions
  • AJNR Editorial Board Alumni

American Society of Neuroradiology

  • Not an ASNR Member? Join Now

© 2025 by the American Society of Neuroradiology All rights, including for text and data mining, AI training, and similar technologies, are reserved.
Print ISSN: 0195-6108 Online ISSN: 1936-959X

Powered by HighWire