Skip to main content
Advertisement

Main menu

  • Home
  • Content
    • Current Issue
    • Publication Preview--Ahead of Print
    • Past Issue Archive
    • Case of the Week Archive
    • Classic Case Archive
    • Case of the Month Archive
    • COVID-19 Content and Resources
  • For Authors
  • About Us
    • About AJNR
    • Editors
    • American Society of Neuroradiology
  • Submit a Manuscript
  • Podcasts
    • Subscribe on iTunes
    • Subscribe on Stitcher
  • More
    • Subscribers
    • Permissions
    • Advertisers
    • Alerts
    • Feedback
  • Other Publications
    • ajnr

User menu

  • Subscribe
  • Alerts
  • Log in

Search

  • Advanced search
American Journal of Neuroradiology
American Journal of Neuroradiology

American Journal of Neuroradiology

  • Subscribe
  • Alerts
  • Log in

Advanced Search

  • Home
  • Content
    • Current Issue
    • Publication Preview--Ahead of Print
    • Past Issue Archive
    • Case of the Week Archive
    • Classic Case Archive
    • Case of the Month Archive
    • COVID-19 Content and Resources
  • For Authors
  • About Us
    • About AJNR
    • Editors
    • American Society of Neuroradiology
  • Submit a Manuscript
  • Podcasts
    • Subscribe on iTunes
    • Subscribe on Stitcher
  • More
    • Subscribers
    • Permissions
    • Advertisers
    • Alerts
    • Feedback
  • Follow AJNR on Twitter
  • Visit AJNR on Facebook
  • Follow AJNR on Instagram
  • Join AJNR on LinkedIn
  • RSS Feeds
Research ArticleFunctional Vignette

Memory Part 1: Overview

F.D. Raslau, A.P. Klein, J.L. Ulmer, V. Mathews and L.P. Mark
American Journal of Neuroradiology November 2014, 35 (11) 2058-2060; DOI: https://doi.org/10.3174/ajnr.A4059
F.D. Raslau
aFrom the University of Kentucky (F.D.R.), Lexington, Kentucky
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
A.P. Klein
bMedical College of Wisconsin (A.P.K., J.L.U., V.M., L.P.M.), Milwaukee, Wisconsin.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
J.L. Ulmer
bMedical College of Wisconsin (A.P.K., J.L.U., V.M., L.P.M.), Milwaukee, Wisconsin.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
V. Mathews
bMedical College of Wisconsin (A.P.K., J.L.U., V.M., L.P.M.), Milwaukee, Wisconsin.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
L.P. Mark
bMedical College of Wisconsin (A.P.K., J.L.U., V.M., L.P.M.), Milwaukee, Wisconsin.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • References
  • PDF
Loading

A common layman's conception of memory is the simple storage and retrieval of learned information that often evokes comparison to a filing system. Our everyday experience of memory, however, is in fact a complicated multifactorial process that consists of both conscious and unconscious components, and depends on the integration of a variety of information from distinct functional systems, each processing different types of information by different areas of the brain (substrates), while working in a concerted fashion.1⇓⇓–4 In other words, memory is not a singular process. It represents an integrated network of neurologic tasks and connections. In this light, memory may evoke comparison with an orchestra composed of many different instruments, each making different sounds and responsible for different parts of the score, but when played together in the proper coordinated fashion, making an integrated musical experience that is greater than the simple sum of the individual instruments.

Memory consists of 2 broad categories (Fig 1): short- and long-term memory.5 Short-term memory is also called working memory. Long-term memory can be further divided into declarative and nondeclarative memory. These are also referred to as explicit/conscious and implicit/unconscious memory, respectively. Declarative or explicit memory consists of episodic (events) and semantic (facts) memory. Nondeclarative or implicit memory consists of priming, skill learning, and conditioning. In everyday experiences (Fig 2), declarative memory refers to things you know that you can tell others, and nondeclarative or procedural memory to things you know that you can show by doing.6 An example of episodic memory is remembering your first day of school, whereas semantic memory is knowing the capital of Canada. Nondeclarative memory manifests as knowing how to ride a bicycle (skill learning), being more likely to use a word that you recently heard (priming), and salivating when you see a favorite food (conditioning).

Fig 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig 1.

Classification of memory.

Fig 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig 2.

The everyday experience of different aspects of memory. Adapted with permission from Breedlove SM, Watson NV, Rosenzweig MR. Biological Psychology: An Introduction to Behavioral, Cognitive, and Clinical Neuroscience. 6th ed. Sunderland, Massachusetts: Sinauer Associates; 2010.

The process of memory is dynamic with continual change over time.5 Memory traces are initially formed as a series of connections between the hippocampus and various cortical areas that are responsible for processing the particular characteristics of an event (Fig 3). Over time, with repetitive conscious retrieval of the memory and/or sleep, connections are formed between the cortical areas relevant for that memory, while the hippocampal connections become less important. Eventually, the hippocampal connections become unnecessary and the cortical connections stand alone as an integrated representation of the memory. This occurs when the memory is consolidated. Figure 4 illustrates a more concrete example of this phenomenon. A particular event (such as a birthday party) with auditory, visual, and spatial information is acquired (encoded). The hippocampus holds a unified representation of the event and the auditory information is distributed to the superior aspect of the temporal lobe, the spatial information is distributed to the parietal lobe (inferior parietal lobule), and the visual information is distributed to the occipital cortex. The memory traces are formed as connections between the hippocampus and these cortical areas. At this point in time, the memory has yet to be consolidated so that retrieval of the memory requires reactivation of the particular connections between the hippocampus and cortical regions. After the memory is consolidated, the cortical-to-cortical connections strengthen and the hippocampal-to-cortical connections weaken or become unnecessary. The memory is then directly accessed from the neocortex, bypassing the hippocampus. A key point is that the early unconsolidated memory consists of hippocampal-to-cortical connections that are relatively rapid forming but transient, whereas the consolidated memory consists of cortical-to-cortical connections that are slow forming and long-lasting.

Fig 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig 3.

Memory consolidation. Connections between the hippocampus and various cortical modules are critical in early memory formation (left). Over time, the cortical modules form connections among themselves and the connections with the hippocampus become less important (center). When the memory is fully consolidated, only the cortical to cortical connections are important and the hippocampus becomes unnecessary (right). Adapted with permission from Frankland PW, Bontempi B. The organization of recent and remote memories. Nat Rev Neurosci 2005;6:119–30.

Fig 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig 4.

Retrieval of nonconsolidated and consolidated memory. A, An event with audio (A), spatial (S), and visual (V) information is encoded. The hippocampus contains a unified representation of the event and forms connections with the relevant cortical areas to process the audio, spatial, and visual information. B, When a retrieval cue containing only spatial and visual information of the event is encountered early in the memory formation process before the memory has been consolidated, the hippocampus plays a critical role by accessing its connections with the pertinent cortical areas for the entire memory. C, After the memory is fully consolidated, the connections with the hippocampus become unnecessary and the retrieval cue accesses the memory directly from the cortical to cortical network of connections that form the unified representation of the memory. Adapted with permission from Purves D, Cabeza R, Huettel SA, et al. Principles of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2nd ed. Sunderland, Massachusetts: Sinauer Associates; 2013.

Théodule Armand Ribot, a noted French psychologist, first described the concept of memory consolidation in 1882.7 He noted a temporal gradient to memory loss after brain damage. The tendency for recent memories to be affected to a greater extent than remote memories has become known as Ribot's law. This commonly observed phenomenon among trauma victims underscores the transient and less stable nature of recent memories that have yet to be fully consolidated, especially if the insult occurs early in the memory processing timeframe. Memories in close chronologic proximity to the traumatic event are often lost, whereas memories that are more remote tend to be less affected. The loss of memory for events that happened before an insult/injury to the brain is called retrograde amnesia. The inability to form new memories after brain injury is called anterograde amnesia. When bilateral hippocampal formation injury occurs, the ability to form new memories can be greatly impeded because of the critical role the hippocampal formation plays in the early formation of memories. Figure 5 is a coronal T1-weighted image from an MR examination performed in 1992 showing bilateral medial temporal lobe injury to the extensively studied patient, H.M., who in 1953 underwent bilateral resection of the entire piriform-amygdaloid-hippocampal complex for seizures and subsequently experienced severe anterograde amnesia.8⇓⇓–11

Fig 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig 5.

Patient H.M. and medial temporal lobe damage. A 1992 coronal T1-weighted image of patient H.M. shows abnormal hippocampal formation hypointense signal after previous bilateral medial temporal lobe resection performed decades earlier for intractable seizures. After the surgery, the patient experienced anterograde amnesia and has been extensively studied for years afterward. Adapted from Corkin S, Amaral DG, González RG, et al. H.M.'s medial temporal lobe lesion: findings from magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosci 1997;17:3964–79.

Many components of the brain (prefrontal cortex, temporal lobe, parietal lobe, occipital lobe, medial diencephalon) are involved in the processes that form memories; however, this review will focus more on the medial temporal lobe and fornix. These medial temporal lobe components consist of the hippocampal formation, parahippocampal gyrus, parahippocampal cortex, entorrhinal cortex, and perirhinal cortex. The contribution of these structures to the function of memory will be the subject of the next article in the Functional Vignettes series.

References

  1. 1.↵
    1. Zola-Morgan S,
    2. Squire LR
    . Neuroanatomy of memory. Annu Rev Neurosci 1993;16:547–63
    CrossRefPubMed
  2. 2.↵
    1. Yancey SW,
    2. Phelps EA
    . Functional neuroimaging and episodic memory: a perspective. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2001;23:32–48
    CrossRefPubMed
  3. 3.↵
    1. Tulving E
    . What is episodic memory? Curr Dir Psychol Sci 1993;2:67–70
    FREE Full Text
  4. 4.↵
    1. Squire LR
    . Memory systems of the brain: a brief history and current perspective. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2004;82:171–77
    CrossRefPubMed
  5. 5.↵
    1. Frankland PW,
    2. Bontempi B
    . The organization of recent and remote memories. Nat Rev Neurosci 2005;6:119–30
    CrossRefPubMed
  6. 6.↵
    1. Breedlove SM,
    2. Watson NV,
    3. Rosenzweig MR
    . Biological Psychology: An Introduction to Behavioral, Cognitive, and Clinical Neuroscience. 6th ed. Sunderland, Massachusetts: Sinauer Associates; 2010
  7. 7.↵
    1. Ribot TA
    . Diseases of Memory: An Essay in the Positive Psychology. The International Scientific Series. New York: D. Appleton and Co; 1882
  8. 8.↵
    1. Corkin S,
    2. Amaral DG,
    3. González RG, et al
    . H.M.'s medial temporal lobe lesion: findings from magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosci 1997;17:3964–79
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  9. 9.↵
    1. Salat DH,
    2. van der Kouwe AJ,
    3. Tuch DS, et al
    . Neuroimaging H.M.: 10-year follow-up examination. Hippocampus 2006;16:936–45
    CrossRefPubMed
  10. 10.↵
    1. Schmolck H,
    2. Kensinger EA,
    3. Corkin S, et al
    . Semantic knowledge in patient H.M. and other patients with bilateral medial and lateral temporal lobe lesions. Hippocampus 2002;12:520–33
    CrossRefPubMed
  11. 11.↵
    1. Kensinger EA,
    2. Ullman MT,
    3. Corkin S
    . Bilateral medial temporal lobe damage does not affect lexical or grammatical processing: evidence from amnesic patient H.M. Hippocampus 2001;11:347–60
    CrossRefPubMed
  • Received April 9, 2014.
  • Accepted after revision April 14, 2014.
  • © 2014 by American Journal of Neuroradiology
PreviousNext
Back to top

In this issue

American Journal of Neuroradiology: 35 (11)
American Journal of Neuroradiology
Vol. 35, Issue 11
1 Nov 2014
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
  • Complete Issue (PDF)
Advertisement
Print
Download PDF
Email Article

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on American Journal of Neuroradiology.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Memory Part 1: Overview
(Your Name) has sent you a message from American Journal of Neuroradiology
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the American Journal of Neuroradiology web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Citation Tools
Memory Part 1: Overview
F.D. Raslau, A.P. Klein, J.L. Ulmer, V. Mathews, L.P. Mark
American Journal of Neuroradiology Nov 2014, 35 (11) 2058-2060; DOI: 10.3174/ajnr.A4059

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Share
Memory Part 1: Overview
F.D. Raslau, A.P. Klein, J.L. Ulmer, V. Mathews, L.P. Mark
American Journal of Neuroradiology Nov 2014, 35 (11) 2058-2060; DOI: 10.3174/ajnr.A4059
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One
Purchase

Jump to section

  • Article
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • References
  • PDF

Related Articles

  • No related articles found.
  • PubMed
  • Google Scholar

Cited By...

  • No citing articles found.
  • Crossref (2)
  • Google Scholar

This article has been cited by the following articles in journals that are participating in Crossref Cited-by Linking.

  • Interictal epileptiform activity outside the seizure onset zone impacts cognition
    Hoameng Ung, Christian Cazares, Ameya Nanivadekar, Lohith Kini, Joost Wagenaar, Danielle Becker, Abba Krieger, Timothy Lucas, Brian Litt, Kathryn A. Davis
    Brain 2017 140 8
  • Integrative Brain Dynamics in Childhood Bullying Victimization: Cognitive and Emotional Convergence Associated With Stress Psychopathology
    Iryna S. Palamarchuk, Tracy Vaillancourt
    Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience 2022 16

More in this TOC Section

  • Memory Part 2: The Role of the Medial Temporal Lobe
Show more Functional Vignette

Similar Articles

Advertisement

News and Updates

  • Lucien Levy Best Research Article Award
  • Thanks to our 2022 Distinguished Reviewers
  • Press Releases

Resources

  • Evidence-Based Medicine Level Guide
  • How to Participate in a Tweet Chat
  • AJNR Podcast Archive
  • Ideas for Publicizing Your Research
  • Librarian Resources
  • Terms and Conditions

Opportunities

  • Share Your Art in Perspectives
  • Get Peer Review Credit from Publons
  • Moderate a Tweet Chat

American Society of Neuroradiology

  • Neurographics
  • ASNR Annual Meeting
  • Fellowship Portal
  • Position Statements

© 2023 by the American Society of Neuroradiology | Print ISSN: 0195-6108 Online ISSN: 1936-959X

Powered by HighWire